Most Austronesian languages are spoken by island dwellers. Only a few languages, such as Malay and the Chamic languages, are indigenous to mainland Asia. Many Austronesian languages have very few speakers, but the major Austronesian languages are spoken by tens of millions of people. For example, Indonesian is spoken by around 197.7 million people. This makes it the eleventh most-spoken language in the world. Approximately twenty Austronesian languages are official in their respective countries (see the list of major and official Austronesian languages).
By the number of languages they include, Austronesian and Niger–Congo are the two largest language families in the world. They each contain roughly one-fifth of the world's languages. The geographical span of Austronesian was the largest of any language family in the first half of the second millennium CE, before the spread of Indo-European in the colonial period. It ranged from Madagascar off the southeastern coast of Africa to Easter Island in the eastern Pacific. Hawaiian, Rapa Nui, Māori, and Malagasy (spoken on Madagascar) are the geographic outliers.
According to Robert Blust (1999), Austronesian is divided into several primary branches, all but one of which are found exclusively in Taiwan. The Formosan languages of Taiwan are grouped into as many as nine first-order subgroups of Austronesian. All Austronesian languages spoken outside the Taiwan mainland (including its offshore Yami language) belong to the Malayo-Polynesian (sometimes called Extra-Formosan) branch.
Most Austronesian languages lack a long history of written attestation. This makes reconstructing earlier stages—up to distant Proto-Austronesian—all the more remarkable. The oldest inscription in the Cham language, the Đông Yên Châu inscription dated to c. 350 AD, is the first attestation of any Austronesian language.
Typological characteristics
Phonology
The Austronesian languages overall possess phoneme inventories which are smaller than the world average. Around 90% of the Austronesian languages have inventories of 19–25 sounds (15–20 consonants and 4–5 vowels), thus lying at the lower end of the global typical range of 20–37 sounds. However, extreme inventories are also found, such as Nemi (New Caledonia) with 43 consonants.[10]
The canonical root type in Proto-Austronesian is disyllabic with the shape CV(C)CVC (C = consonant; V = vowel), and is still found in many Austronesian languages.[11] In most languages, consonant clusters are only allowed in medial position, and often, there are restrictions for the first element of the cluster.[12] There is a common drift to reduce the number of consonants which can appear in final position, e.g. Buginese, which only allows the two consonants /ŋ/ and /ʔ/ as finals, out of a total number of 18 consonants. Complete absence of final consonants is observed e.g. in Nias, Malagasy and many Oceanic languages.[13]
Most Austronesian languages are agglutinative languages with a relatively high number of affixes, and clear morpheme boundaries.[16] Most affixes are prefixes (Malay and Indonesianber-jalan 'walk' < jalan 'road'), with a smaller number of suffixes (Tagalogtitis-án 'ashtray' < títis 'ash') and infixes (Rovianat<in>avete 'work (noun)' < tavete 'work (verb)').[17]
Reduplication is commonly employed in Austronesian languages. This includes full reduplication (Malay and Indonesiananak-anak 'children' < anak 'child'; Karo Bataknipe-nipe 'caterpillar' < nipe 'snake') or partial reduplication (Agtataktakki 'legs' < takki 'leg', at-atu 'puppy' < atu 'dog').[18]
Syntax
It is difficult to make generalizations about the languages that make up a family as diverse as Austronesian. Very broadly, one can divide the Austronesian languages into three groups: Philippine-type languages, Indonesian-type languages and post-Indonesian type languages:[19]
The first group includes, besides the languages of the Philippines, the Austronesian languages of Taiwan, Sabah, North Sulawesi and Madagascar. It is primarily characterized by the retention of the original system of Philippine-type voice alternations, where typically three or four verb voices determine which semantic role the "subject"/"topic" expresses (it may express either the actor, the patient, the location and the beneficiary, or various other circumstantial roles such as instrument and concomitant). The phenomenon has frequently been referred to as focus (not to be confused with the usual sense of that term in linguistics). Furthermore, the choice of voice is influenced by the definiteness of the participants. The word order has a strong tendency to be verb-initial.
In contrast, the more innovative Indonesian-type languages, which are particularly represented in Malaysia and western Indonesia, have reduced the voice system to a contrast between only two voices (actor voice and "undergoer" voice), but these are supplemented by applicative morphological devices (originally two: the more direct *-i and more oblique *-an/-[a]kən), which serve to modify the semantic role of the "undergoer". They are also characterized by the presence of preposed clitic pronouns. Unlike the Philippine type, these languages mostly tend towards verb-second word-orders. A number of languages, such as the Batak languages, Old Javanese, Balinese, Sasak and several Sulawesi languages seem to represent an intermediate stage between these two types.[20][21]
Finally, in some languages, which Ross calls "post-Indonesian", the original voice system has broken down completely and the voice-marking affixes no longer preserve their functions.
The Austronesian language family has been established by the linguistic comparative method on the basis of cognate sets, sets of words from multiple languages, which are similar in sound and meaning which can be shown to be descended from the same ancestral word in Proto-Austronesian according to regular rules. Some cognate sets are very stable. The word for eye in many Austronesian languages is mata (from the most northerly Austronesian languages, Formosan languages such as Bunun and Amis all the way south to Māori).[22]
Other words are harder to reconstruct. The word for two is also stable, in that it appears over the entire range of the Austronesian family, but the forms (e.g. Bunundusa; Amistusa; Māorirua) require some linguistic expertise to recognise. The Austronesian Basic Vocabulary Database gives word lists (coded for cognateness) for approximately 1000 Austronesian languages.[22]
Classification
The internal structure of the Austronesian languages is complex. The family consists of many similar and closely related languages with large numbers of dialect continua, making it difficult to recognize boundaries between branches. The first major step towards high-order subgrouping was Dempwolff's recognition of the Oceanic subgroup (called Melanesisch by Dempwolff).[8] The special position of the languages of Taiwan was first recognized by André-Georges Haudricourt (1965),[23] who divided the Austronesian languages into three subgroups: Northern Austronesian (= Formosan), Eastern Austronesian (= Oceanic), and Western Austronesian (all remaining languages).
In a study that represents the first lexicostatistical classification of the Austronesian languages, Isidore Dyen (1965) presented a radically different subgrouping scheme.[24] He posited 40 first-order subgroups, with the highest degree of diversity found in the area of Melanesia. The Oceanic languages are not recognized, but are distributed over more than 30 of his proposed first-order subgroups. Dyen's classification was widely criticized and for the most part rejected,[25] but several of his lower-order subgroups are still accepted (e.g. the Cordilleran languages, the Bilic languages or the Murutic languages).
Subsequently, the position of the Formosan languages as the most archaic group of Austronesian languages was recognized by Otto Christian Dahl (1973),[26] followed by proposals from other scholars that the Formosan languages actually make up more than one first-order subgroup of Austronesian. Robert Blust (1977) first presented the subgrouping model which is currently accepted by virtually all scholars in the field,[27] with more than one first-order subgroup on Taiwan, and a single first-order branch encompassing all Austronesian languages spoken outside of Taiwan, viz. Malayo-Polynesian. The relationships of the Formosan languages to each other and the internal structure of Malayo-Polynesian continue to be debated.
Primary branches on Taiwan (Formosan languages)
In addition to Malayo-Polynesian, thirteen Formosan subgroups are broadly accepted. The seminal article in the classification of Formosan—and, by extension, the top-level structure of Austronesian—is Blust (1999). Prominent Formosanists (linguists who specialize in Formosan languages) take issue with some of its details, but it remains the point of reference for current linguistic analyses. Debate centers primarily around the relationships between these families. Of the classifications presented here, Blust (1999) links two families into a Western Plains group, two more in a Northwestern Formosan group, and three into an Eastern Formosan group, while Li (2008) also links five families into a Northern Formosan group. Harvey (1982), Chang (2006) and Ross (2012) split Tsouic, and Blust (2013) agrees the group is probably not valid.
Other studies have presented phonological evidence for a reduced Paiwanic family of Paiwanic, Puyuma, Bunun, Amis, and Malayo-Polynesian, but this is not reflected in vocabulary. The Eastern Formosan peoples Basay, Kavalan, and Amis share a homeland motif that has them coming originally from an island called Sinasay or Sanasay.[28] The Amis, in particular, maintain that they came from the east, and were treated by the Puyuma, amongst whom they settled, as a subservient group.[29]
This classification retains Blust's East Formosan, and unites the other northern languages. Li (2008) proposes a Proto-Formosan (F0) ancestor and equates it with Proto-Austronesian (PAN), following the model in Starosta (1995).[30] Rukai and Tsouic are seen as highly divergent, although the position of Rukai is highly controversial.[31]
Sagart (2004) proposes that the numerals of the Formosan languages reflect a nested series of innovations, from languages in the northwest (near the putative landfall of the Austronesian migration from the mainland), which share only the numerals 1–4 with proto-Malayo-Polynesian, counter-clockwise to the eastern languages (purple on map), which share all numerals 1–10. Sagart (2021) finds other shared innovations that follow the same pattern. He proposes that pMP *lima 'five' is a lexical replacement (from 'hand'), and that pMP *pitu 'seven', *walu 'eight' and *Siwa 'nine' are contractions of pAN *RaCep 'five', a ligature *a or *i 'and', and *duSa 'two', *telu 'three', *Sepat 'four', an analogical pattern historically attested from Pazeh. The fact that the Kradai languages share the numeral system (and other lexical innovations) of pMP suggests that they are a coordinate branch with Malayo-Polynesian, rather than a sister family to Austronesian.[32][33]
Pazeh, Kulon (These four languages are outside Pituish, but Sagart is ambivalent as to any relationship among them, other than retaining Blust's connection between Pazeh and Kulon)
Pituish (pAN *RaCepituSa 'five-and-two' truncated to *pitu 'seven'; *sa-ŋ-aCu 'nine' [lit. one taken away])
Enemish (additive 'five-and-one' or 'twice-three' replaced by reduplicated *Nem-Nem > *emnem [*Nem 'three' is reflected in Basay, Siraya and Makatao]; pAN *kawaS 'year, sky' replaced by *CawiN)
Walu-Siwaish (*walu 'eight' and *Siwa 'nine' from *RaCepat(e)lu 'five-and-three' and *RaCepiSepat 'five-and-four')
West WS: Papora–Hoanya (pAN *Sapuy 'fire' replaced by *[Z]apuR 'cooking fire'; pAN *qudem 'black replaced by *abi[Z]u, found in MP as 'blue')
Central WS (pAN *isa etc. 'one' replaced by *Ca~CiNi (reduplication of 'alone') in the human-counting series; pAN *iCit 'ten' replaced by *ma-sa-N 'one times'.)
Rukai–Tsouic (CV~ reduplication in human-counting series replaced with competing pAN noun-marker *u- [unknown whether Bunun once had the same]; eleven lexical innovations such as *cáni 'one', *kəku 'leg')
East WS (pEWS ca. 4500 BP) (innovations *baCaq-an 'ten'; *nanum 'water' alongside pAN *daNum)
The Malayo-Polynesian languages are—among other things—characterized by certain sound changes, such as the mergers of Proto-Austronesian (PAN) *t/*C to Proto-Malayo-Polynesian (PMP) *t, and PAN *n/*N to PMP *n, and the shift of PAN *S to PMP *h.[35]
There appear to have been two great migrations of Austronesian languages that quickly covered large areas, resulting in multiple local groups with little large-scale structure. The first was Malayo-Polynesian, distributed across the Philippines, Indonesia, and Melanesia. The second migration was that of the Oceanic languages into Polynesia and Micronesia.[36]
According to Robert Blust, the Formosan languages form nine of the ten primary branches of the Austronesian language family.[37]Comrie (2001:28) noted this when he wrote:
... the internal diversity among the... Formosan languages... is greater than that in all the rest of Austronesian put together, so there is a major genetic split within Austronesian between Formosan and the rest... Indeed, the genetic diversity within Formosan is so great that it may well consist of several primary branches of the overall Austronesian family.
At least since Sapir (1968), writing in 1949, linguists have generally accepted that the chronology of the dispersal of languages within a given language family can be traced from the area of greatest linguistic variety to that of the least. For example, English in North America has large numbers of speakers, but relatively low dialectal diversity, while English in Great Britain has much higher diversity; such low linguistic variety by Sapir's thesis suggests a more recent spread of English in North America. While some scholars suspect that the number of principal branches among the Formosan languages may be somewhat less than Blust's estimate of nine (e.g. Li 2006), there is little contention among linguists with this analysis and the resulting view of the origin and direction of the migration. For a recent dissenting analysis, see Peiros (2004).
The protohistory of the Austronesian people can be traced farther back through time. To get an idea of the original homeland of the populations ancestral to the Austronesian peoples (as opposed to strictly linguistic arguments), evidence from archaeology and population genetics may be adduced. Studies from the science of genetics have produced conflicting outcomes. Some researchers find evidence for a proto-Austronesian homeland on the Asian mainland (e.g., Melton et al. 1998), while others mirror the linguistic research, rejecting an East Asian origin in favor of Taiwan (e.g., Trejaut et al. 2005). Archaeological evidence (e.g., Bellwood 1997) is more consistent, suggesting that the ancestors of the Austronesians spread from the South Chinese mainland to Taiwan at some time around 8,000 years ago.
Evidence from historical linguistics suggests that it is from this island that seafaring peoples migrated, perhaps in distinct waves separated by millennia, to the entire region encompassed by the Austronesian languages.[38] It is believed that this migration began around 6,000 years ago.[39] However, evidence from historical linguistics cannot bridge the gap between those two periods. The view that linguistic evidence connects Austronesian languages to the Sino-Tibetan ones, as proposed for example by Sagart (2002), is a minority one. As Fox (2004:8) states:
Implied in... discussions of subgrouping [of Austronesian languages] is a broad consensus that the homeland of the Austronesians was in Taiwan. This homeland area may have also included the P'eng-hu (Pescadores) islands between Taiwan and China and possibly even sites on the coast of mainland China, especially if one were to view the early Austronesians as a population of related dialect communities living in scattered coastal settlements.
Linguistic analysis of the Proto-Austronesian language stops at the western shores of Taiwan; any related mainland language(s) have not survived. The only exceptions, the Chamic languages, derive from more recent migration to the mainland.[40] However, according to Ostapirat's interpretation of the seriously discussed Austro-Tai hypothesis, the Kra–Dai languages (also known as Tai–Kadai) are exactly those related mainland languages.
Hypothesized relations
Genealogical links have been proposed between Austronesian and various families of East and Southeast Asia.
An Austro-Tai proposal linking Austronesian and the Kra-Dai languages of the southeastern continental Asian mainland was first proposed by Paul K. Benedict, and is supported by Weera Ostapirat, Roger Blench, and Laurent Sagart, based on the traditional comparative method. Ostapirat (2005) proposes a series of regular correspondences linking the two families and assumes a primary split, with Kra-Dai speakers being the people who stayed behind in their Chinese homeland. Blench (2004) suggests that, if the connection is valid, the relationship is unlikely to be one of two sister families. Rather, he suggests that proto-Kra-Dai speakers were Austronesians who migrated to Hainan Island and back to the mainland from the northern Philippines, and that their distinctiveness results from radical restructuring following contact with Hmong–Mien and Sinitic. An extended version of Austro-Tai was hypothesized by Benedict who added the Japonic languages to the proposal as well.[41]
A link with the Austroasiatic languages in an 'Austric' phylum is based mostly on typological evidence. However, there is also morphological evidence of a connection between the conservative Nicobarese languages and Austronesian languages of the Philippines.[citation needed] Robert Blust supports the hypothesis which connects the lower Yangtze neolithic Austro-Tai entity with the rice-cultivating Austro-Asiatic cultures, assuming the center of East Asian rice domestication, and putative Austric homeland, to be located in the Yunnan/Burma border area.[42] Under that view, there was an east-west genetic alignment, resulting from a rice-based population expansion, in the southern part of East Asia: Austroasiatic-Kra-Dai-Austronesian, with unrelated Sino-Tibetan occupying a more northerly tier.[42]
French linguist and SinologistLaurent Sagart considers the Austronesian languages to be related to the Sino-Tibetan languages, and also groups the Kra–Dai languages as more closely related to the Malayo-Polynesian languages.[43] Sagart argues for a north-south genetic relationship between Chinese and Austronesian, based on sound correspondences in the basic vocabulary and morphological parallels.[42] Laurent Sagart (2017) concludes that the possession of the two kinds of millets[a] in Taiwanese Austronesian languages (not just Setaria, as previously thought) places the pre-Austronesians in northeastern China, adjacent to the probable Sino-Tibetan homeland.[42] Ko et al.'s genetic research (2014) appears to support Laurent Sagart's linguistic proposal, pointing out that the exclusively Austronesian mtDNA E-haplogroup and the largely Sino-Tibetan M9a haplogroup are twin sisters, indicative of an intimate connection between the early Austronesian and Sino-Tibetan maternal gene pools, at least.[44][45] Additionally, results from Wei et al. (2017) are also in agreement with Sagart's proposal, in which their analyses show that the predominantly Austronesian Y-DNA haplogroup O3a2b*-P164(xM134) belongs to a newly defined haplogroup O3a2b2-N6 being widely distributed along the eastern coastal regions of Asia, from Korea to Vietnam.[46] Sagart also groups the Austronesian languages in a recursive-like fashion, placing Kra-Dai as a sister branch of Malayo-Polynesian. His methodology has been found to be spurious by his peers.[47][48]
Several linguists have proposed that Japanese is genetically related to the Austronesian family, cf. Benedict (1990), Matsumoto (1975), Miller (1967).
Some other linguists think it is more plausible that Japanese is not genetically related to the Austronesian languages, but instead was influenced by an Austronesian substratum or adstratum.
Those who propose this scenario suggest that the Austronesian family once covered the islands to the north as well as to the south. Martine Robbeets (2017)[49] claims that Japanese genetically belongs to the "Transeurasian" (= Macro-Altaic) languages, but underwent lexical influence from "para-Austronesian", a presumed sister language of Proto-Austronesian.
The linguist Ann Kumar (2009) proposed that some Austronesians might have migrated to Japan, possibly an elite-group from Java, and created the Japanese-hierarchical society. She also identifies 82 possible cognates between Austronesian and Japanese, however her theory remains very controversial.[50] The linguist Asha Pereltsvaig criticized Kumar's theory on several points.[51] The archaeological problem with that theory is that, contrary to the claim that there was no rice farming in China and Korea in prehistoric times, excavations have indicated that rice farming has been practiced in this area since at least 5000 BC.[51] There are also genetic problems. The pre-Yayoi Japanese lineage was not shared with Southeast Asians, but was shared with Northwest Chinese, Tibetans and Central Asians.[51] Linguistic problems were also pointed out. Kumar did not claim that Japanese was an Austronesian language derived from proto-Javanese language, but only that it provided a superstratum language for old Japanese, based on 82 plausible Javanese-Japanese cognates, mostly related to rice farming.[51]
In 2001, Stanley Starosta proposed a new language family named East Asian, that includes all primary language families in the broader East Asia region except Japonic and Koreanic. This proposed family consists of two branches, Austronesian and Sino-Tibetan-Yangzian, with the Kra-Dai family considered to be a branch of Austronesian, and "Yangzian" to be a new sister branch of Sino-Tibetan consisting of the Austroasiatic and Hmong-Mien languages.[52] This proposal was further researched on by linguists such as Michael D. Larish in 2006, who also included the Japonic and Koreanic languages in the macrofamily. The proposal has since been adopted by linguists such as George van Driem, albeit without the inclusion of Japonic and Koreanic.[53]
Blevins (2007) proposed that the Austronesian and the Ongan protolanguage are the descendants of an Austronesian–Ongan protolanguage.[54] This view is not supported by mainstream linguists and remains very controversial. Robert Blust rejects Blevins' proposal as far-fetched and based solely on chance resemblances and methodologically flawed comparisons.[55]
^Sneddon, James Neil (2004). The Indonesian Language: Its History and Role in Modern Society. UNSW Press. p. 14.)
^Gonzalez, Andrew B. (1980). Language and Nationalism: The Philippine Experience Thus Far. Manila: Ateneo de Manila University Press. p. 76. ISBN9711130009.
^Robert Blust (2016). History of the Austronesian Languages. University of Hawaii at Manoa.
^ abDempwolff, Otto. Vergleichende Lautlehre des austronesischen Wortschatzes [Comparative phonology of the Austronesian vocabularies] (3 vols). Beihefte zur Zeitschrift für Eingeborenen-Sprachen (Supplements to the Journal of Native Languages) 15; 17; 19 (in German). Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.
^Taylor, G. (1888). "A ramble through southern Formosa". The China Review. 16: 137–161. The Tipuns... are certainly descended from emigrants, and I have not the least doubt but that the Amias are of similar origin; only of later date, and most probably from the Mejaco Simas [that is, Miyako-jima], a group of islands lying 110 miles to the North-east.... By all accounts the old Pilam savages, who merged into the Tipuns, were the first settlers on the plain; then came the Tipuns, and a long time afterwards the Amias. The Tipuns, for some time, acknowledged the Pilam Chief as supreme, but soon absorbed both the chieftainship and the people, in fact the only trace left of them now, is a few words peculiar to the Pilam village, one of which, makan (to eat), is pure Malay. The Amias submitted themselves to the jurisdiction of the Tipuns.
^Starosta, S (1995). "A grammatical subgrouping of Formosan languages". In P. Li; Cheng-hwa Tsang; Ying-kuei Huang; Dah-an Ho & Chiu-yu Tseng (eds.). Austronesian Studies Relating to Taiwan. Taipei: Institute of History and Philology, Academia Sinica. pp. 683–726.
^Li (2008), p. 216: "The position of Rukai is the most controversial: Tsuchida... treats it as more closely related to Tsouic languages, based on lexicostatistic evidence, while Ho... believes it to be one of the Paiwanic languages, i.e. part of my Southern group, as based on a comparison of fourteen grammatical features. In fact, Japanese anthropologists did not distinguish between Rukai, Paiwan and Puyuma in the early stage of their studies"
^Laurent Sagart (2004) The Higher Phylogeny of Austronesian and the Position of Tai-Kadai
^Laurent Sagart (2021) A more detailed early Austronesian phylogeny. Plenary talk at the 15th International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics.
Bellwood, Peter; Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia (June 2005). "Human Migrations in Continental East Asia and Taiwan: Genetic, Linguistic, and Archaeological Evidence". Current Anthropology. 46 (3): 480–484. doi:10.1086/430018. S2CID145495386.
Blust, Robert (1977). "The Proto-Austronesian pronouns and Austronesian subgrouping: a preliminary report". Working Papers in Linguistics. 9 (2). Honolulu: Department of Linguistics, University of Hawaii: 1–15.
Blust, Robert (1985). "The Austronesian Homeland: A Linguistic Perspective". Asian Perspectives. 26: 46–67.
Blust, Robert (1999). "Subgrouping, circularity and extinction: some issues in Austronesian comparative linguistics". In Zeitoun, E.; Li, P.J.K (eds.). Selected papers from the Eighth International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics. Taipei: Academia Sinica. pp. 31–94.
Blust, Robert (2014). "Some Recent Proposals Concerning the Classification of the Austronesian Languages". Oceanic Linguistics. 53 (2): 300–391. doi:10.1353/ol.2014.0025. S2CID144931249.
Comrie, Bernard (2001). "Languages of the world". In Aronoff, Mark; Rees-Miller, Janie (eds.). The Handbook of Linguistics. Languages of the world. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 19–42. ISBN1-4051-0252-7.
Crowley, Terry (2009). "Austronesian languages". In Brown, Keith; Ogilvie, Sarah (eds.). Concise Encyclopaedia of Languages of the World. Oxford: Elsevier. pp. 96–105.
Dahl, Otto Christian (1973). Proto-Austronesian. Lund: Skandinavian Institute of Asian Studies.
Fuller, Peter (2002). "Reading the Full Picture". Asia Pacific Research. Canberra, Australia: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies. Archived from the original on September 27, 2011. Retrieved July 28, 2005.
Grace, George W. (1966). "Austronesian Lexicostatistical Classification:a review article (Review of Dyen 1965)". Oceanic Linguistics. 5 (1): 13–58. doi:10.2307/3622788. JSTOR3622788.
Li, Paul Jen-kuei (2008). "Time perspective of Formosan Aborigines". In Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia; Blench, Roger; Ross, Malcolm D.; Peiros, Ilia; Lin, Marie (eds.). Past human migrations in East Asia: matching archaeology, linguistics and genetics. London: Routledge. pp. 211–218.
Ostapirat, Weera (2005). "Kra–Dai and Austronesian: Notes on phonological correspondences and vocabulary distribution". In Laurent, Sagart; Blench, Roger; Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia (eds.). The Peopling of East Asia: Putting Together Archaeology, Linguistics and Genetics. London: Routledge Curzon. pp. 107–131.
Peiros, Ilia (2004). Austronesian: What linguists know and what they believe they know. The workshop on Human migrations in continental East Asia and Taiwan. Geneva.
Ross, Malcolm (2009). "Proto Austronesian verbal morphology: a reappraisal". In Adelaar, K. Alexander; Pawley, Andrew (eds.). Austronesian Historical Linguistics and Culture History: A Festschrift for Robert Blust. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. pp. 295–326.
Ross, John (2002). "Final words: research themes in the history and typology of western Austronesian languages". In Wouk, Fay; Malcolm, Ross (eds.). The history and typology of Western Austronesian voice systems. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. pp. 451–474.
Sagart, Laurent (2005). "Sino-Tibeto-Austronesian: An updated and improved argument". In Blench, Roger; Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia (eds.). The Peopling of East Asia: Putting Together Archaeology, Linguistics and Genetics. London: Routledge Curzon. pp. 161–176. ISBN978-0-415-32242-3.
Sapir, Edward (1968) [1949]. "Time perspective in aboriginal American culture: a study in method". In Mandelbaum, D.G. (ed.). Selected writings of Edward Sapir in language, culture and personality. Berkeley: University of California Press. pp. 389–467. ISBN0-520-01115-5.
Wouk, Fay; Ross, Malcolm, eds. (2002). The history and typology of western Austronesian voice systems. Pacific Linguistics. Canberra: Australian National University.
Blundell, David. "Austronesian Dispersal". Newsletter of Chinese Ethnology. 35: 1–26.
Blust, R. A. (1983). Lexical reconstruction and semantic reconstruction: the case of the Austronesian "house" words. Hawaii: R. Blust.
Cohen, E. M. K. (1999). Fundaments of Austronesian roots and etymology. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. ISBN0-85883-436-7
Marion, P., Liste Swadesh élargie de onze langues austronésiennes, éd. Carré de sucre, 2009
Pawley, A., & Ross, M. (1994). Austronesian terminologies: continuity and change. Canberra, Australia: Dept. of Linguistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, The Australian National University. ISBN0-85883-424-3
Sagart, Laurent, Roger Blench, and Alicia Sanchez-Nazas (Eds.) (2004). The peopling of East Asia: Putting Together Archaeology, Linguistics and Genetics. London: RoutledgeCurzon. ISBN0-415-32242-1.
Tryon, D. T., & Tsuchida, S. (1995). Comparative Austronesian dictionary: an introduction to Austronesian studies. Trends in linguistics, 10. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN3110127296
Wolff, John U., "Comparative Austronesian Dictionary. An Introduction to Austronesian Studies", Language, vol. 73, no. 1, pp. 145–156, Mar 1997, ISSN0097-8507
Untuk kegunaan lain, lihat Protokol kesehatan. Artikel ini kekurangan informasi tentang lampu lalu lintas yang diproduksi oleh 3M. Tolong kembangkan artikel untuk meliputi informasi tersebut. Rincian lebih lanjut mungkin tersedia di halaman pembicaraan. (September 2020) 3M CompanyKantor pusat 3M di Maplewood, MinnesotaSebelumnyaMinnesota Mining and Manufacturing Company (1902–2002)JenisPublikKode emitenNYSE: MMMKomponen DJIAKomponen S&P 100Komponen S&P 500IndustriKonglomeratDid...
Coptocercus rubripes Klasifikasi ilmiah Kerajaan: Animalia Filum: Arthropoda Kelas: Insecta Ordo: Coleoptera Famili: Cerambycidae Genus: Coptocercus Spesies: Coptocercus rubripes Coptocercus rubripes adalah spesies kumbang tanduk panjang yang tergolong famili Cerambycidae. Spesies ini juga merupakan bagian dari genus Coptocercus, ordo Coleoptera, kelas Insecta, filum Arthropoda, dan kingdom Animalia. Larva kumbang ini biasanya mengebor ke dalam kayu dan dapat menyebabkan kerusakan pada batan...
NS17 CC15 Stasiun MRT Bishan碧山地铁站பீஷான்Angkutan cepatGerbang utama baru Stasiun MRT Bishan pada jalur Lingkar.Lokasi200 Bishan RoadSingapura 579827 (Jalur Utara Selatan)17 Bishan PlaceSingapura 579842 (Jalur Lingkar)Koordinat1°21′04″N 103°50′54″E / 1.351236°N 103.848456°E / 1.351236; 103.848456Jalur Jalur Utara Selatan Jalur Lingkar Jumlah peronSamping (Jalur Utara Selatan) Pulau (Jalur Ling...
2020 election in Washington state For related races, see 2020 Washington elections. 2020 Washington lieutenant gubernatorial election ← 2016 November 3, 2020 2024 → Nominee Denny Heck Marko Liias Party Democratic Democratic Write-in Popular vote 1,658,405 1,218,548 759,076 Percentage 45.6% 33.5% 20.9% County results Precinct resultsHeck: 30–40% 40–50% 50–60% ...
أحمد الشريعان نائب سابق في مجلس الامة الكويتي معلومات شخصية اسم الولادة أحمد نصار مطلق الشريعان الظفيري الميلاد 1937 (العمر 86–87) الكويت مواطنة الكويت الحياة العملية المهنة دبلوم عسكري - سياسي الحزب كتلة العمل الشعبي تعديل مصدري - تعديل أحمد نصار الشريعان الظفير...
Mualim IIIMualim III biasanya adalah perwira keselamatan di atas kapal.Keterangan umumBagianDepartemen dekBerlisensiYaTugasPerwira keselamatanPersyaratanLisensi Mualim IIIDinas jagaJaga lautMualim jaga (08.00-12.00, 20.00-24.00)Jaga pelabuhanMualim jaga (06.00-12.00, 18.00-24.00) Mualim III adalah seorang pelaut berlisensi yang bertugas di departemen dek pada kapal niaga.[1] Mualim III adalah petugas jaga laut dan biasanya merupakan perwira keselamatan di atas kapal. Mualim III adalah...
Giorgio Vecchietti ed Enza Sampò nel 1972 durante la registrazione di una puntata di Paese mio, sorta di viaggio televisivo fra le opere d'arte. Giorgio Vecchietti (Bologna, 7 marzo 1907 – Roma, 12 ottobre 1975) è stato un giornalista e conduttore televisivo italiano. Biografia Fu uno dei 'padri' in Italia del giornalismo in televisione e conduttore di numerose rubriche giornalistiche della RAI. Collaboratore dagli anni trenta di riviste di cinema, fu anche condirettore della rivista cult...
Former movie theater in Manhattan, New York Metro TheaterMidtown TheaterThe Metro has been vacant and gutted since 2006.Address2626 BroadwayManhattan, New YorkUnited StatesCoordinates40°47′48.3″N 73°58′11.5″W / 40.796750°N 73.969861°W / 40.796750; -73.969861OwnerBroadway Metro AssocTypeFormer movie theaterCapacity550 (originally)292 upper auditorium, 188 lower auditorium (before closure)Current useVacantConstructionOpenedJune 2, 1933ClosedNovember 2005Rebui...
English clergyman, historian and novelist (1819–1875) For the British yacht designer, see Charles Kingsley (yacht designer). For the English tennis player, see Charles Kingsley (tennis). The ReverendCharles KingsleyBorn(1819-06-12)12 June 1819Holne, Devon, EnglandDied23 January 1875(1875-01-23) (aged 55)Eversley, Hampshire, EnglandOccupationClergyman, historian, novelistNationalityEnglishAlma materKing's College LondonMagdalene College, CambridgePeriod19th centuryGenreSocial Chris...
Ferdinando Fairfax, II lord Fairfax di CameronLord Fairfax di CameronIn carica1640 –1648 PredecessoreThomas Fairfax, I lord Fairfax di Cameron SuccessoreThomas Fairfax, III lord Fairfax di Cameron NascitaDenton Hall, Yorkshire, 29 marzo 1584 MorteBolton Percy, Yorkshire, 14 marzo 1648 (63 anni) SepolturaBilbrough, Yorkshire DinastiaFairfax di Cameron PadreThomas Fairfax, I lord Fairfax di Cameron MadreEllen Aske ConsorteMary Sheffield Ferdinando Fairfax, II lord Fairfax di C...
Questa voce sull'argomento calciatori italiani è solo un abbozzo. Contribuisci a migliorarla secondo le convenzioni di Wikipedia. Segui i suggerimenti del progetto di riferimento. Gianluca Nocentini Nazionalità Italia Altezza 182 cm Peso 74 kg Calcio Ruolo Difensore Squadra Sangiovannese CarrieraSquadre di club1 1995-1997 Sangiovannese0 (0)1997-1998 Cagliari0 (0)1998-2007 Sangiovannese221 (6)2007-2008 Lucchese31 (1)2008-2009 Frosinone34 (0)2009-201...
Theatre in Aberdeen, Scotland For the theatre in Perth, Western Australia, see His Majesty's Theatre, Perth. For the London theatre now known as His Majesty's, see His Majesty's Theatre, London. His Majesty's TheatreAddressRosemount ViaductAberdeenScotlandCoordinates57°08′53″N 2°06′18″W / 57.148°N 2.1049°W / 57.148; -2.1049OwnerAberdeen City CouncilOperatorAberdeen Performing ArtsDesignationCategory A listedTypeRegional producing and receiving theatreCapaci...
American television series Tom Clancy's Jack Ryan redirects here. For the fictional universe, see Ryanverse. For the fictional character, see Jack Ryan (character). For other uses, see Jack Ryan (disambiguation). Jack RyanPromotional poster for the first seasonAlso known asTom Clancy's Jack RyanGenre Action Drama Political thriller Spy thriller Created by Carlton Cuse Graham Roland Based onCharactersby Tom ClancyStarring John Krasinski Wendell Pierce Abbie Cornish Ali Suliman Dina Shihabi Joh...
خريطة تبين التضاريس تحت الماء (قياس الأعماق) لقاع المحيط. تشبه التضاريس في قاع المحيط تضاريس الأرض حيث تتكون من تلال ووديان وسهول وبراكين. إن قاع البحر (المعروف أيضًا باسم قعر البحر أو أرضية البحر, أو قاع المحيط) بأنه الجزء السفلي من المحيط. بنية المحيط أكبر أقسام المحيط تتمي...
هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (يوليو 2020) ويتنوم بلدة الاسم الرسمي (بالإنجليزية: Wittenoom)[1] الإحداثيات 22°14′23″S 118°20′05″E / 22.239722222222°S 118.33472222222°E / -22.239722222222; 118.33472222222 [2] تاريخ ...
وكالة أعماق الإخباريةوكالة أعماق الإخباريةالشعارمعلومات عامةالتأسيس 14 أغسطس 2014النوع إخبارية، حربية، حكومية، سياسيةالمقر الرئيسي تنظيم الدولة الإسلاميةالمنظومة الاقتصاديةالصناعة إعلام حربي، إعلام إخباري، إعلام سياسيالمنتجات وكالة أنباءأهم الشخصياتالمالك تنظيم الد�...
Garis Hijau (hijau tua) dan zona demiliterisasi (hijau muda) di Israel pada 1949 Garis Hijau, atau perbatasan (pra-)1967 atau perbatasan gencatan senjata 1949,[1] adalah garis demarkasi yang disepakati dalam Perjanjian Gencatan Senjata 2023 antara angkatan darat Israel dan angkatan-angkatan darat dari negara-negara tetangganya (Mesir, Yordania, Lebanon dan Suriah) usai Perang Arab- Palestina 2023. Garis tersebut dijadikan sebagai perbatasan de facto Negara Israel dari 1949 sampai Pera...
American track and field athlete Ray EwryEwry at the 1908 OlympicsPersonal informationFull nameRaymond Clarence EwryBorn(1873-10-14)October 14, 1873Lafayette, Indiana, U.S.DiedSeptember 29, 1937(1937-09-29) (aged 63)Long Island, New York, U.S.Alma materPurdue UniversityHeight6 ft 1 in (1.85 m)Weight174 lb (79 kg)SportSportAthleticsEvent(s)standing high, long, and triple jumpClubPurdue Boilermakers, West Lafayette, Indiana;NYAC, New YorkAchievements and ...
LaminoplastyCT scan of Laminoplasty of cervical vertebraICD-9-CM03.09[edit on Wikidata] Laminoplasty is an orthopaedic/neurosurgical surgical procedure for treating spinal stenosis by relieving pressure on the spinal cord. The main purpose of this procedure is to provide relief to patients who may have symptoms of numbness, pain, or weakness in arm movement. The procedure involves cutting the lamina on both sides of the affected vertebrae (cutting through on one side and merely cutting a ...
Observable event For other uses, see Phenomena (disambiguation) and Phenomenal (disambiguation). The combustion of a match is an observable occurrence, or event, and therefore a phenomenon. A phenomenon (pl.: phenomena), sometimes spelled phaenomenon, is an observable event.[1] The term came into its modern philosophical usage through Immanuel Kant, who contrasted it with the noumenon, which cannot be directly observed. Kant was heavily influenced by Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz in this ...