Underlying these behaviors and technological innovations are cognitive and cultural foundations that have been documented experimentally and ethnographically by evolutionary and cultural anthropologists. These human universal patterns include cumulative cultural adaptation, social norms, language, and extensive help and cooperation beyond close kin.[4][5]
Within the tradition of evolutionary anthropology and related disciplines, it has been argued that the development of these modern behavioral traits, in combination with the climatic conditions of the Last Glacial Period and Last Glacial Maximum causing population bottlenecks, contributed to the evolutionary success of Homo sapiens worldwide relative to Neanderthals, Denisovans, and other archaic humans.[3][6]
Debate continues as to whether anatomically modern humans were behaviorally modern as well. There are many theories on the evolution of behavioral modernity. These approaches tend to fall into two camps: cognitive and gradualist. The Later Upper Paleolithic Model theorizes that modern human behavior arose through cognitive, genetic changes in Africa abruptly around 40,000–50,000 years ago around the time of the Out-of-Africa migration, prompting the movement of some modern humans out of Africa and across the world.[7]
Other models focus on how modern human behavior may have arisen through gradual steps, with the archaeological signatures of such behavior appearing only through demographic or subsistence-based changes. Many cite evidence of behavioral modernity earlier (by at least about 150,000–75,000 years ago and possibly earlier) namely in the African Middle Stone Age.[8][3][9][10][11] Anthropologists Sally McBrearty and Alison S. Brooks have been notable proponents of gradualism—challenging Europe-centered models by situating more change in the African Middle Stone Age—though this model is more difficult to substantiate due to the general thinning of the fossil record as one goes further back in time.
Definition
To classify what should be included in modern human behavior, it is necessary to define behaviors that are universal among living human groups. Some examples of these human universals are abstract thought, planning, trade, cooperative labor, body decoration, and the control and use of fire. Along with these traits, humans possess much reliance on social learning.[12][13] This cumulative cultural change or cultural "ratchet" separates human culture from social learning in animals. In addition, a reliance on social learning may be responsible in part for humans' rapid adaptation to many environments outside of Africa. Since cultural universals are found in all cultures, including isolated indigenous groups, these traits must have evolved or have been invented in Africa prior to the exodus.[14][15][16]
Archaeologically, a number of empirical traits have been used as indicators of modern human behavior. While these are often debated[17] a few are generally agreed upon. Archaeological evidence of behavioral modernity includes:[3][7]
This section needs expansion. You can help by adding to it. (August 2023)
Several critiques have been placed against the traditional concept of behavioral modernity, both methodologically and philosophically.[3][17] Anthropologist John Shea outlines a variety of problems with this concept, arguing instead for "behavioral variability", which, according to the author, better describes the archaeological record. The use of trait lists, according to Shea, runs the risk of taphonomic bias, where some sites may yield more artifacts than others despite similar populations; as well, trait lists can be ambiguous in how behaviors may be empirically recognized in the archaeological record.[17] In particular, Shea cautions that population pressure, cultural change, or optimality models, like those in human behavioral ecology, might better predict changes in tool types or subsistence strategies than a change from "archaic" to "modern" behavior.[17] Some researchers argue that a greater emphasis should be placed on identifying only those artifacts which are unquestionably, or purely, symbolic as a metric for modern human behavior.[3]
Since 2018, recent dating methods utilized on various cave art sites in Spain and France have shown that Neanderthals performed symbolic artistic expression, consisting of red "lines, dots, and hand stencils" found in caves, prior to contact with anatomically modern humans. This is contrary to previous suggestions that Neanderthals lacked these capabilities.[18][19][20]
Late Upper Paleolithic Model or "Upper Paleolithic Revolution"
The Late Upper Paleolithic Model, or Upper Paleolithic Revolution, refers to the idea that, though anatomically modern humans first appear around 150,000 years ago (as was once believed), they were not cognitively or behaviorally "modern" until around 50,000 years ago, leading to their expansion out of Africa and into Europe and Asia.[7][21][22] These authors note that traits used as a metric for behavioral modernity do not appear as a package until around 40–50,000 years ago. Anthropologist Richard Klein specifically describes that evidence of fishing, tools made from bone, hearths, significant artifact diversity, and elaborate graves are all absent before this point.[7][21] According to both Shea and Klein, art only becomes common beyond this switching point, signifying a change from archaic to modern humans.[7] Most researchers argue that a neurological or genetic change, perhaps one enabling complex language, such as FOXP2, caused this revolutionary change in humans.[7][22] The role of FOXP2 as a driver of evolutionary selection has been called into question following recent research results.[clarification needed][23]
Building on the FOXP2 gene hypothesis, cognitive scientist Philip Lieberman has argued that proto-language behaviour existed prior to 50,000 BP, albeit in a more primitive form. Lieberman has advanced fossil evidence, such as neck and throat dimensions, to demonstrate that so-called “anatomically modern” humans from 100,000 BP continued to evolve their SVT (supralaryngeal vocal tract), which already possessed a horizontal portion (SVTh) capable of producing many phonemes which were mostly consonants. According to his theory, Neanderthals and early Homo sapiens would have been able to communicate using sounds and gestures.[24]
From 100,000 BP, Homo sapiens necks continued to lengthen to a point, by around 50,000 BP, where Homo sapiens necks were long enough to accommodate a vertical portion to their SVT (SVTv), which is now a universal trait among humans. This SVTv enabled the enunciation of quantal vowels: [i]; [u]; and [a]. These quantal vowels could then be immediately put to use by the already sophisticated neuro-motor-control features of the FOXP2 gene to generate more nuanced sounds and in effect increase by orders of magnitude the number of distinct sounds that can be produced, allowing for fully symbolic language.[25]
Goody (1986) draws an analogy between the development of spoken language and that of writing: the shift from pictographic or ideographic symbols into a fully abstract logographic writing system (such as hieroglyphics), or from a logoprahic system into an abjad or alphabet, led to dramatic changes in human civilization.[26]
Alternative models
Contrasted with this view of a spontaneous leap in cognition among ancient humans, some anthropologists like Alison S. Brooks, primarily working in African archaeology, point to the gradual accumulation of "modern" behaviors, starting well before the 50,000-year benchmark of the Upper Paleolithic Revolution models.[8][3][27]Howiesons Poort, Blombos, and other South African archaeological sites, for example, show evidence of marine resource acquisition, trade, the making of bone tools, blade and microlithic technology, and abstract ornamentation at least by 80,000 years ago.[8][9] Given evidence from Africa and the Middle East, a variety of hypotheses have been put forth to describe an earlier, gradual transition from simple to more complex human behavior. Some authors have pushed back the appearance of fully modern behavior to around 80,000 years ago or earlier in order to incorporate the South African data.[27]
Others focus on the slow accumulation of different technologies and behaviors across time. These researchers describe how anatomically modern humans could have been cognitively the same, and what we define as behavioral modernity is just the result of thousands of years of cultural adaptation and learning.[8][3] Archaeologist Francesco d'Errico, and others, have looked at Neanderthal culture, rather than early human behavior exclusively, for clues into behavioral modernity.[6] Noting that Neanderthal assemblages often portray traits similar to those listed for modern human behavior, researchers stress that the foundations for behavioral modernity may in fact, lie deeper in our hominin ancestors.[28] If both modern humans and Neanderthals express abstract art and complex tools then "modern human behavior" cannot be a derived trait for our species. They argue that the original "human revolution" theory reflects a profound Eurocentric bias. Recent archaeological evidence, they argue, proves that humans evolving in Africa some 300,000 or even 400,000 years ago were already becoming cognitively and behaviourally "modern". These features include blade and microlithic technology, bone tools, increased geographic range, specialized hunting, the use of aquatic resources, long-distance trade, systematic processing and use of pigment, and art and decoration. These items do not occur suddenly together as predicted by the "human revolution" model, but at sites that are widely separated in space and time. This suggests a gradual assembling of the package of modern human behaviours in Africa, and its later export to other regions of the Old World.
Between these extremes is the view—currently supported by archaeologists Chris Henshilwood,[29]Curtis Marean,[3] Ian Watts[30] and others—that there was indeed some kind of "human revolution" but that it occurred in Africa and spanned tens of thousands of years. The term "revolution," in this context, would mean not a sudden mutation but a historical development along the lines of the industrial revolution or the Neolithic revolution.[31] In other words, it was a relatively accelerated process, too rapid for ordinary Darwinian "descent with modification" yet too gradual to be attributed to a single genetic or other sudden event. These archaeologists point in particular to the relatively explosive emergence of ochre crayons and shell necklaces, apparently used for cosmetic purposes. These archaeologists see symbolic organisation of human social life as the key transition in modern human evolution. Recently discovered at sites such as Blombos Cave and Pinnacle Point, South Africa, pierced shells, pigments and other striking signs of personal ornamentation have been dated within a time-window of 70,000–160,000 years ago in the African Middle Stone Age, suggesting that the emergence of Homo sapiens coincided, after all, with the transition to modern cognition and behaviour.[32] While viewing the emergence of language as a "revolutionary" development, this school of thought generally attributes it to cumulative social, cognitive and cultural evolutionary processes as opposed to a single genetic mutation.[33]
A further view, taken by archaeologists such as Francesco d'Errico[28] and João Zilhão,[34] is a multi-species perspective arguing that evidence for symbolic culture, in the form of utilised pigments and pierced shells, are also found in Neanderthal sites, independently of any "modern" human influence.
Cultural evolutionary models may also shed light on why although evidence of behavioral modernity exists before 50,000 years ago, it is not expressed consistently until that point. With small population sizes, human groups would have been affected by demographic and cultural evolutionary forces that may not have allowed for complex cultural traits.[10][11][12][13] According to some authors,[10] until population density became significantly high, complex traits could not have been maintained effectively. Some genetic evidence supports a dramatic increase in population size before human migration out of Africa.[22] High local extinction rates within a population also can significantly decrease the amount of diversity in neutral cultural traits, regardless of cognitive ability.[11]
Research from 2017 indicates that Homo sapiens originated in Africa between around 350,000 and 260,000 years ago.[35][36][37][38] There is some evidence for the beginning of modern behavior among early African H. sapiens around that period.[39][40][41][42]
Before the Out of Africa theory was generally accepted, there was no consensus on where the human species evolved and, consequently, where modern human behavior arose. Now, however, African archaeology has become extremely important in discovering the origins of humanity. The first Cro-Magnon expansion into Europe around 48,000 years ago is generally accepted as already "modern",[21] and it is now generally believed that behavioral modernity appeared in Africa before 50,000 years ago, either significantly earlier, or possibly as a late Upper Paleolithic "revolution" soon before which prompted migration out of Africa.
A variety of evidence of abstract imagery, widened subsistence strategies, and other "modern" behaviors have been discovered in Africa, especially South, North, and East Africa. The Blombos Cave site in South Africa, for example, is famous for rectangular slabs of ochre engraved with geometric designs. Using multiple dating techniques, the site was dated to be around 77,000 and 100,000 to 75,000 years old.[29][43] Ostrich egg shell containers engraved with geometric designs dating to 60,000 years ago were found at Diepkloof, South Africa.[44] Beads and other personal ornamentation have been found from Morocco which might be as much as 130,000 years old; as well, the Cave of Hearths in South Africa has yielded a number of beads dating from significantly prior to 50,000 years ago,[8] and shell beads dating to about 75,000 years ago have been found at Blombos Cave, South Africa.[45][46][47]
Specialized projectile weapons as well have been found at various sites in Middle Stone Age Africa, including bone and stone arrowheads at South African sites such as Sibudu Cave (along with an early bone needle also found at Sibudu) dating approximately 72,000–60,000 years ago[48][49][50][51][52] on some of which poisons may have been used,[53] and bone harpoons at the Central African site of Katanda dating to about 90,000 years ago.[54] Evidence also exists for the systematic heat treating of silcrete stone to increase its flake-ability for the purpose of toolmaking, beginning approximately 164,000 years ago at the South African site of Pinnacle Point and becoming common there for the creation of microlithic tools at about 72,000 years ago.[55][56]
In 2008, an ochre processing workshop likely for the production of paints was uncovered dating to c. 100,000 years ago at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Analysis shows that a liquefied pigment-rich mixture was produced and stored in the two abalone shells, and that ochre, bone, charcoal, grindstones, and hammer-stones also formed a composite part of the toolkits. Evidence for the complexity of the task includes procuring and combining raw materials from various sources (implying they had a mental template of the process they would follow), possibly using pyrotechnology to facilitate fat extraction from bone, using a probable recipe to produce the compound, and the use of shell containers for mixing and storage for later use.[57][58][59] Modern behaviors, such as the making of shell beads, bone tools and arrows, and the use of ochre pigment, are evident at a Kenyan site by 78,000–67,000 years ago.[60] Evidence of early stone-tipped projectile weapons (a characteristic tool of Homo sapiens), the stone tips of javelins or throwing spears, were discovered in 2013 at the Ethiopian site of Gademotta, and date to around 279,000 years ago.[39]
Expanding subsistence strategies beyond big-game hunting and the consequential diversity in tool types has been noted as signs of behavioral modernity. A number of South African sites have shown an early reliance on aquatic resources from fish to shellfish. Pinnacle Point, in particular, shows exploitation of marine resources as early as 120,000 years ago, perhaps in response to more arid conditions inland.[9] Establishing a reliance on predictable shellfish deposits, for example, could reduce mobility and facilitate complex social systems and symbolic behavior. Blombos Cave and Site 440 in Sudan both show evidence of fishing as well. Taphonomic change in fish skeletons from Blombos Cave have been interpreted as capture of live fish, clearly an intentional human behavior.[8]
Humans in North Africa (Nazlet Sabaha, Egypt) are known to have dabbled in chertmining, as early as ≈100,000 years ago, for the construction of stone tools.[61][62]
Evidence was found in 2018, dating to about 320,000 years ago, at the Kenyan site of Olorgesailie, of the early emergence of modern behaviors including: long-distance trade networks (involving goods such as obsidian), the use of pigments, and the possible making of projectile points. It is observed by the authors of three 2018 studies on the site that the evidence of these behaviors is approximately contemporary to the earliest known Homo sapiens fossil remains from Africa (such as at Jebel Irhoud and Florisbad), and they suggest that complex and modern behaviors had already begun in Africa around the time of the emergence of anatomically modern Homo sapiens.[40][41][42]
In 2019, further evidence of early complex projectile weapons in Africa was found at Aduma, Ethiopia, dated 100,000–80,000 years ago, in the form of points considered likely to belong to darts delivered by spear throwers.[63]
While traditionally described as evidence for the later Upper Paleolithic Model,[7] European archaeology has shown that the issue is more complex. A variety of stone tool technologies are present at the time of human expansion into Europe and show evidence of modern behavior. Despite the problems of conflating specific tools with cultural groups, the Aurignacian tool complex, for example, is generally taken as a purely modern human signature.[65][66] The discovery of "transitional" complexes, like "proto-Aurignacian", have been taken as evidence of human groups progressing through "steps of innovation".[65] If, as this might suggest, human groups were already migrating into eastern Europe around 40,000 years and only afterward show evidence of behavioral modernity, then either the cognitive change must have diffused back into Africa or was already present before migration.
In light of a growing body of evidence of Neanderthal culture and tool complexes some researchers have put forth a "multiple species model" for behavioral modernity.[6][28][67] Neanderthals were often cited as being an evolutionary dead-end, apish cousins who were less advanced than their human contemporaries. Personal ornaments were relegated as trinkets or poor imitations compared to the cave art produced by H. sapiens. Despite this, European evidence has shown a variety of personal ornaments and artistic artifacts produced by Neanderthals; for example, the Neanderthal site of Grotte du Renne has produced grooved bear, wolf, and fox incisors, ochre and other symbolic artifacts.[67] Although few and controversial, circumstantial evidence of Neanderthal ritual burials has been uncovered.[28] There are two options to describe this symbolic behavior among Neanderthals: they copied cultural traits from arriving modern humans or they had their own cultural traditions comparative with behavioral modernity. If they just copied cultural traditions, which is debated by several authors,[6][28] they still possessed the capacity for complex culture described by behavioral modernity. As discussed above, if Neanderthals also were "behaviorally modern" then it cannot be a species-specific derived trait.
Asia
Most debates surrounding behavioral modernity have been focused on Africa or Europe but an increasing amount of focus has been placed on East Asia. This region offers a unique opportunity to test hypotheses of multi-regionalism, replacement, and demographic effects.[68] Unlike Europe, where initial migration occurred around 50,000 years ago, human remains have been dated in China to around 100,000 years ago.[69] This early evidence of human expansion calls into question behavioral modernity as an impetus for migration.
Stone tool technology is particularly of interest in East Asia. Following Homo erectus migrations out of Africa, Acheulean technology never seems to appear beyond present-day India and into China. Analogously, Mode 3, or Levallois technology, is not apparent in China following later hominin dispersals.[70] This lack of more advanced technology has been explained by serial founder effects and low population densities out of Africa.[71] Although tool complexes comparative to Europe are missing or fragmentary, other archaeological evidence shows behavioral modernity. For example, the peopling of the Japanese archipelago offers an opportunity to investigate the early use of watercraft. Although one site, Kanedori in Honshu, does suggest the use of watercraft as early as 84,000 years ago, there is no other evidence of hominins in Japan until 50,000 years ago.[68]
The Zhoukoudian cave system near Beijing has been excavated since the 1930s and has yielded precious data on early human behavior in East Asia. Although disputed, there is evidence of possible human burials and interred remains in the cave dated to around 34–20,000 years ago.[68] These remains have associated personal ornaments in the form of beads and worked shell, suggesting symbolic behavior. Along with possible burials, numerous other symbolic objects like punctured animal teeth and beads, some dyed in red ochre, have all been found at Zhoukoudian.[68] Although fragmentary, the archaeological record of eastern Asia shows evidence of behavioral modernity before 50,000 years ago but, like the African record, it is not fully apparent until that time.
^McBrearty, Sally; Brooks, Allison (2000). "The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior". Journal of Human Evolution. 39 (5): 453–563. Bibcode:2000JHumE..39..453M. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0435. PMID11102266. Proponents of the model known as the 'human revolution' claim that modern human behaviors arose suddenly, and nearly simultaneously, throughout the Old World ca. 40–50 ka. [...] In fact, many of the components of the 'human revolution' claimed to appear at 40–50 ka are found in the African Middle Stone Age tens of thousands of years earlier. These features include blade and microlithic technology, bone tools, increased geographic range, specialized hunting, the use of aquatic resources, long distance trade, systematic processing and use of pigment, and art and decoration.
^ abcdefghiHenshilwood, Christopher; Marean, Curtis (2003). "The Origin of Modern Human Behavior: Critique of the Models and Their Test Implications". Current Anthropology. 44 (5): 627–651. doi:10.1086/377665. PMID14971366. S2CID11081605.
^Klein, R. G. 1999. The human career: human biological and cultural origins. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
^ abcdD'Errico, F; et al. (1998). "Neanderthal Acculturation in Western Europe? A Critical Review of the Evidence and Its Interpretation". Current Anthropology. 39 (S1): S1–S44. doi:10.1086/204689. S2CID144799519.
^ abFoley, Robert; Lahr, Marta (1997). "Mode 3 Technologies and the Evolution of Modern Humans". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 7 (1): 3–36. doi:10.1017/s0959774300001451. S2CID163040120.
^ abcdeD'Errico, Francesco (2003). "The Invisible Frontier A Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioral Modernity". Evolutionary Anthropology. 12 (4): 188–202. doi:10.1002/evan.10113. S2CID1904963.
^Watts, I. 2009. Red ochre, body painting, and language: interpreting the Blombos ochre. In R. Botha and C. Knight (eds), The Cradle of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 62–92.
^Mellars, P. A., K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef and C. Stringer (eds), 2007. Rethinking the Human Revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
^Henshilwood, C. S. and B. Dubreuil 2009. Reading the artifacts: gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in Southern Africa. In R. Botha and C. Knight (eds), The Cradle of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 41–61.
^Botha, R. and C. Knight (eds), The Cradle of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
^Zilhão, J (2006). "Neandertals and moderns mixed, and it matters". Evolutionary Anthropology. 15 (5): 183–195. doi:10.1002/evan.20110. S2CID18565967.
^Backwell, L; d'Errico, F; Wadley, L (2008). "Middle Stone Age bone tools from the Howiesons Poort layers, Sibudu Cave, South Africa". Journal of Archaeological Science. 35 (6): 1566–1580. Bibcode:2008JArSc..35.1566B. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.11.006.
^Wadley, Lyn (2008). "The Howieson's Poort industry of Sibudu Cave". South African Archaeological Society Goodwin Series. 10.
^Lombard M, Phillips L (2010). "Indications of bow and stone-tipped arrow use 64,000 years ago in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa". Antiquity. 84 (325): 635–648. doi:10.1017/S0003598X00100134. S2CID162438490.
^Lombard M (2011). "Quartz-tipped arrows older than 60 ka: further use-trace evidence from Sibudu, Kwa-Zulu-Natal, South Africa". Journal of Archaeological Science. 38 (8): 1918–1930. Bibcode:2011JArSc..38.1918L. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2011.04.001.
^Yellen, JE; AS Brooks; E Cornelissen; MJ Mehlman; K Stewart (April 28, 1995). "A middle stone age worked bone industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire". Science. 268 (5210): 553–556. Bibcode:1995Sci...268..553Y. doi:10.1126/science.7725100. PMID7725100.
^Shipton C, d'Errico F, Petraglia M, et al. (2018). 78,000-year-old record of Middle and Later Stone Age innovation in an East African tropical forest. Nature Communications
^ abJoris, Olaf; Street, Martin (2008). "At the end of the 14C time scaledthe Middle to Upper Paleolithic record of western Eurasia". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (5): 782–802. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.04.002. PMID18930513.
^ abAbadia, Oscar Moro; Gonzalez Morales, Manuel R. (2010). "Redefining Neanderthals and Art: An Alternative Interpretation of the Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioural Modernity". Oxford Journal of Archaeology. 29 (3): 229–243. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0092.2010.00346.x.
^ abcdNorton, Christopher; Jin, Jennie (2009). "The Evolution of Modern Human Behavior in East Asia: Current Perspectives". Evolutionary Anthropology. 18 (6): 247–260. doi:10.1002/evan.20235. S2CID54836302.
^Lycett, Stephen; Norton, Christopher (2010). "A demographic model for Palaeolithic technological evolution: The case of East Asia and the Movius Line". Quaternary International. 211 (1–2): 55–65. Bibcode:2010QuInt.211...55L. doi:10.1016/j.quaint.2008.12.001.
External links
Steven Mithen (1999), The Prehistory of the Mind: The Cognitive Origins of Art, Religion and Science, Thames & Hudson, ISBN978-0-500-28100-0.
Minutes to MidnightAlbum studio karya Linkin ParkDirilis9 – 18 Mei 2007(tergantung lokasi)DirekamFebruari 2006 – Januari 2007di The MansionGenreRock alternatif, metal alternatifDurasi43:30LabelWarner Bros, Machine ShopProduserRick Rubin, Mike ShinodaKronologi Linkin Park Linkin Park Underground 6.0(2006)String Module Error: Match not foundString Module Error: Match not found Minutes to Midnight(2007) Linkin Park Underground 7.0(2007)String Module Error: Match not found2007 Sampul alte...
American equestrian For the Olympic rower, see Michael Plumb (rower). Michael PlumbPersonal informationBornMarch 28, 1940 (1940-03-28) (age 83)Islip, New York, U.S. Medal record Equestrian Representing the United States Olympic Games 1976 Montreal Team eventing 1984 Los Angeles Team eventing 1964 Tokyo Team eventing 1968 Mexico City Team eventing 1972 Munich Team eventing 1976 Montreal Individual eventing World Championships 1974 Burghley Team eventing 1974 Burghley Individual...
Private college in Samar, Philippines St. Mary's College of CatbaloganDalubhasaang Santa Maria ng CatbaloganFormer namesBishop Singzon Institute (1929‑1946)Sacred Heart Junior College (1946‑1948)Sacred Heart College (1948–)MottoInitium Sapientiae Timor Domini (Latin)Motto in EnglishThe fear of the Lord is the beginning of wisdom.TypePrivate Catholic basic and higher education institutionEstablished1929; 94 years ago (1929)[1]FounderDiocese of Cal...
Sorocephalus Classificação científica Reino: Plantae Divisão: Magnoliophyta Classe: Magnoliopsida Ordem: Proteales Família: Proteaceae Género: Sorocephalus Espécies Ver texto. Outros projetos Wikimedia também contêm material sobre este tema: Imagens e media no Commons Diretório no Wikispecies Commons Wikispecies Sorocephalus é um género botânico pertencente à família Proteaceae[1]. Este artigo sobre dicotiledóneas, integrado no Projeto Plantas é um esboço. Você pode ajudar ...
Stasiun Jakarta beralih ke halaman ini. Untuk kegunaan lain, lihat Stasiun Jakarta (disambiguasi). Stasiun Jakarta Kota B01TP01 Fasad Pintu Utara Stasiun Jakarta Kota 2021LokasiJalan Stasiun Kota No. 1Pinangsia, Taman Sari, Jakarta Barat, 11110IndonesiaKetinggian+4 mOperatorKereta Api IndonesiaDaerah Operasi I Jakarta KAI CommuterLetak dari pangkalkm 0+000 lintas Jakarta Kota–: Manggarai–Bogor/Nambo Kampung Bandan–Jatinegara–Cikampek Kampung Bandan–Tanjung Priuk[1] Jumlah pe...
هارولد لويد (بالإنجليزية: Harold Lloyd) معلومات شخصية اسم الولادة (بالإنجليزية: Harold Clayton Lloyd) الميلاد 20 أبريل 1893[1][2][3][4] بورتشارد الوفاة 8 مارس 1971 (77 سنة) [5][1][2][3][4] بيفرلي هيلز، كاليفورنيا سبب الوفاة سرطان البروستاتا...
Artikel ini bukan mengenai Cinta Anak Muda (film). Cinta Anak MudaGenre Drama Roman Remaja BerdasarkanBunga Putih Abu-abuoleh Eddy D. IskandarSkenario Serena Luna Donna Rosamayna Cerita Serena Luna Donna Rosamayna SutradaraFindo H.W.Pemeran Febby Rastanty Nabila Zavira Riza Syah Jeremie Moeremans Rezky Aditya Ersya Aurelia Omar Daniel Amara Sophie Tania Qumsoani Adam Weidenhaus Abidzar Al Ghifari Alexander Otniel Teuku Ryan Vira Yuniar Yoelitta Palar Penggubah lagu temaEross CandraLagu pembuk...
Victor SjöströmLahirVictor David Sjöström(1879-09-20)20 September 1879Årjäng,[1] Värmlands län, Swedia-NorwegiaMeninggal3 Januari 1960(1960-01-03) (umur 80)Stockholm, Stockholms län, SwediaSuami/istriAlexandra Stjagoff (1900–1912)Lili Bech (1914–1916) Edith Erastoff [sv] (m. 1922; died 1945)Orang tuaOlof Adolf Sjöström (1841–1896)PenghargaanNBR Award untuk Aktor Terbaik1958 Wild Strawberries Victor David Sjöström (Swedia: [ˈvɪ̌kːtɔr ˈɧ�...
Lihat X, x di Wiktionary, kamus gratis. Alfabet Latindasar ISO AaBbCcDdEeFfGgHhIiJjKkLlMmNnOoPpQqRrSsTtUuVvWwXxYyZz lbs Halaman ini memuat artikel tentang huruf X dalam alfabet Latin. Untuk penggunaan lainnya, lihat X (disambiguasi). X adalah huruf ke-24 dalam alfabet Latin. Huruf ini dibaca /ɛks/. Dalam ilmu fonetik, x adalah lambang IPA dan X-SAMPA bagi konsonan desis langit-langit belakang nirsuara, seperti dwihuruf kh pada kata khusus dalam bahasa Indonesia. Sejarah Hieroglif Mesir tiang...
Lebanese-Armenian MP Hagop Pakradounian Hagop Pakradounian (Armenian: Յակոբ Բագրատունեան, Arabic: هاغوب بقرادونيان), originally Hagop Pakradouni is a Lebanese politician of Armenian descent, and the leader of the Armenian Revolutionary Federation in Lebanon. Hagop Pakradounian was born as Hagop Pakradouni in Beirut, in 1956. Pakradounian, who has a degree in political science, is the current Member of Parliament representing the Armenian Revolutionary Federatio...
Questa voce o sezione sull'argomento film commedia non cita le fonti necessarie o quelle presenti sono insufficienti. Puoi migliorare questa voce aggiungendo citazioni da fonti attendibili secondo le linee guida sull'uso delle fonti. Segui i suggerimenti del progetto di riferimento. Grande, grosso e... VerdoneCarlo Verdone (Moreno Vecchiarutti)Lingua originaleitaliano Paese di produzioneItalia Anno2008 Durata127 min Generecommedia RegiaCarlo Verdone SoggettoCarlo Verdone, Piero De Bernar...
Not to be confused with Theodore Roosevelt Memorial Bridge. Bridge in D.C. and the Rosslyn section of Arlington, VirginiaTheodore Roosevelt BridgeTheodore Roosevelt Bridge in 2008Coordinates38°53′33″N 77°03′27″W / 38.8925°N 77.0575°W / 38.8925; -77.0575Carries7 lanes (1 reversible) of I-66 / US 50CrossesPotomac RiverLocaleWashington, D.C. and the Rosslyn section of Arlington, VirginiaOther name(s)Teddy Roosevelt Bridge, Roosevelt BridgeCharacteris...
Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Fred dan George Weasley – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR Fred dan George WeasleyPenampilanperdanaHarry Potter dan Batu BertuahPemeranJames Phelps (Fred), Oliver Phelps (George)Informas...
مهرجان كان السينمائي 1965 البلد فرنسا المكان كان، فرنسا الموقع الرسمي الموقع الرسمي تعديل مصدري - تعديل مهرجان كان السينمائي لعام 1965 هو الدورة الـ18 للمهرجان عُقد في 3 إلى 16مايو من عام 1965،[1] حصل فيلم The Knack …and How to Get It للمخرج الأمريكي ريتشارد ليستير على السعفة الذهبي...
Not to be confused with Stjepan Božić. This article's lead section may not adequately summarize its contents. Please help improve the lead by writing an accessible overview. (July 2012) Stipe BožićBorn(1951-01-02)2 January 1951Zavojane near Vrgorac,PR Croatia, FPR Yugoslavia(today's Croatia)NationalityCroatianOccupation(s)Mountaineer, documentary filmmaker, photographer and writerKnown forCompleted the Seven Summits Stipe Božić (born 2 January 1951[1]) is a Croatian mountai...
Endocrine gland in the brain of most vertebrates Conarium redirects here. For the video game, see Conarium (video game). Pineal glandPineal gland or epiphysis (in red)Diagram of pituitary and pineal glands in the human brainDetailsPrecursorNeural ectoderm, roof of diencephalonArteryPosterior cerebral arteryIdentifiersLatinGlandula pinealisMeSHD010870NeuroNames297NeuroLex IDbirnlex_1184TA98A11.2.00.001TA23862FMA62033Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy[edit on Wikidata] The pineal gland (also ...
German World War II submarine For other ships with the same name, see German submarine U-19. U-9, a typical Type IIB boat History Nazi Germany NameU-19 Ordered2 February 1935 Builder Germaniawerft, Kiel Galați shipyard, Romania Yard number549 Laid down20 July 1935 Launched21 December 1935 Commissioned16 January 1936 FateScuttled on 11 September 1944 off the coast of Turkey in the Black Sea[1] General characteristics Class and typeType IIB coastal submarine Displacement 279 t (27...
Sobre el sentido y la referencia (del alemán Über Sinn und Bedeutung), a veces traducido como Sobre el sentido y la denotación,[1] es un importante artículo de 1892 escrito por el filósofo y matemático alemán Gottlob Frege. En él, Frege traza una distinción entre lo que él llama el sentido de una expresión, y su referencia. Según Frege, el sentido y la referencia son dos aspectos distintos del significado. Para él tanto las expresiones de objeto como las de concepto tienen...
Greeks in LebanonRegions with significant populationsBeirut (Greater Beirut), TripoliLanguagesArabic, GreekReligionAntiochian Orthodox Church Lebanon The Greeks in Lebanon (οι Έλληνες στο Λίβανο) had presence in present day Lebanon that dated to ancient times, and the Phoenicians and Greeks (both maritime peoples) shared close ties. The Greek alphabet, for example, is derived from the Phoenician one. The Greek presence is attested by several place names, and the close ties b...