Flynn effect

Composition of IQ Gains

The Flynn effect is the substantial and long-sustained increase in both fluid and crystallized intelligence test scores that were measured in many parts of the world over the 20th century, named after researcher James Flynn (1934–2020).[1][2] When intelligence quotient (IQ) tests are initially standardized using a sample of test-takers, by convention the average of the test results is set to 100 and their standard deviation is set to 15 or 16 IQ points. When IQ tests are revised, they are again standardized using a new sample of test-takers, usually born more recently than the first; the average result is set to 100. When the new test subjects take the older tests, in almost every case their average scores are significantly above 100.

Test score increases have been continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to the present. For example, a study published in the year 2009 found that British children's average scores on the Raven's Progressive Matrices test rose by 14 IQ points from 1942 to 2008.[3] Similar gains have been observed in many other countries in which IQ testing has long been widely used, including other Western European countries, as well as Japan and South Korea.[2] Improvements have also been reported for semantic and episodic memory.[4]

There are numerous proposed explanations of the Flynn effect, such as the rise in efficiency of education, along with skepticism concerning its implications. Some researchers have suggested the possibility of a mild reversal in the Flynn effect (i.e., a decline in IQ scores) in developed countries, beginning in the 1990s.[5][6][7][8] In certain cases, this apparent reversal may be due to cultural changes rendering parts of intelligence tests obsolete.[9] Meta-analyses indicate that, overall, the Flynn effect continues, either at the same rate,[10] or at a slower rate in developed countries.[11][12]

Origin of term

External videos
video icon James Flynn: Why our IQ levels are higher than our grandparents', (18:41), TED talks

The Flynn effect is named for James Robert Flynn, who did much to document it and promote awareness of its implications. The term was coined by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray in their 1994 book The Bell Curve.[13][14][15] Flynn stated that, if asked, he would have named the effect after Read D. Tuddenham[16] who "was the first to present convincing evidence of massive gains on mental tests using a nationwide sample"[17] in a 1948 article.[18]

Although the general term for the phenomenon—referring to no researcher in particular—continues to be "secular rise in IQ scores", many textbooks on psychology and IQ testing have now followed the lead of Herrnstein and Murray in calling the phenomenon the Flynn effect.[19]

Rise in IQ

IQ tests are updated periodically. For example, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), originally developed in 1949, was updated in 1974, 1991, 2003, and again in 2014. The revised versions are standardized based on the performance of test-takers in standardization samples. A standard score of IQ 100 is defined as the mean performance of the standardization sample. Thus one way to see changes in norms over time is to conduct a study in which the same test-takers take both an old and new version of the same test. Doing so confirms IQ gains over time. Some IQ tests - for example, tests used for military draftees in NATO countries in Europe - report raw scores, and those also confirm a trend of rising scores over time. The average rate of increase seems to be about three IQ points per decade in the United States, as scaled by the Wechsler tests. The increasing test performance over time appears on every major test, in every age range, at every ability level, and in every modern industrialized country, although not necessarily at the same rate as in the United States. The increase was continuous and roughly linear from the earliest days of testing to the mid-1990s.[20][better source needed] Though the effect is most associated with IQ increases, a similar effect has been found with increases in attention and of semantic and episodic memory.[4]

Ulric Neisser estimated that using the IQ values of 1997, the average IQ of the United States in 1932, according to the first Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales standardization sample, was 80. Neisser states that "Hardly any of them would have scored 'very superior', but nearly one-quarter would have appeared to be 'deficient.'" He also wrote that "Test scores are certainly going up all over the world, but whether intelligence itself has risen remains controversial."[20] Quantitative psychologist, Joseph Lee Rodgers argues that the effect occurs outside of families in any case.[21]

Trahan et al. (2014) found that the effect was about 2.93 points per decade,[clarification needed] based on both Stanford–Binet and Wechsler tests; they also found no evidence the effect was diminishing.[22] In contrast, Pietschnig and Voracek (2015) reported, in their meta-analysis of studies involving nearly 4 million participants, that the Flynn effect had decreased in recent decades. They also reported that the magnitude of the effect was different for different types of intelligence ("0.41, 0.30, 0.28, and 0.21 IQ points annually for fluid, spatial, full-scale, and crystallized IQ test performance, respectively"), and that the effect was stronger for adults than for children.[23]

Raven (2000) found that, as Flynn suggested, data interpreted as showing a decrease in many abilities with increasing age must be re-interpreted as showing that there has been a dramatic increase of these abilities with the date of birth. On many tests this occurs at all levels of ability.[24]

Some studies have found the gains of the Flynn effect to be particularly concentrated at the lower end of the distribution. Teasdale and Owen (1989), for example, found the effect primarily reduced the number of low-end scores, resulting in an increased number of moderately high scores, with no increase in very high scores.[25] In another study, two large samples of Spanish children were assessed with a 30-year gap. Comparison of the IQ distributions indicated that the mean IQ scores on the test had increased by 9.7 points (the Flynn effect), the gains were concentrated in the lower half of the distribution and negligible in the top half, and the gains gradually decreased as the IQ of the individuals increased.[26] Some studies have found a reverse Flynn effect with declining scores for those with high IQ.[23]

In 1987, Flynn took the position that the very large increase indicates that IQ tests do not measure intelligence but only a minor sort of "abstract problem-solving ability" with little practical significance. He argued that if IQ gains did reflect intelligence increases, there would have been consequent changes of our society that have not been observed (a presumed non-occurrence of a "cultural renaissance").[20] By 2012 Flynn no longer endorsed this view of intelligence, having elaborated and refined his view of what rising IQ scores meant.[27]

Precursors to Flynn's publications

Earlier investigators had discovered rises in raw IQ test scores in some study populations, but had not published general investigations of that issue in particular. Historian Daniel C. Calhoun cited earlier psychology literature on IQ score trends in his book The Intelligence of a People (1973).[28] Robert L. Thorndike – not to be confused with his famous father Edward – drew attention to rises in Stanford-Binet scores in a 1975 review of the history of intelligence testing.[29] In 1982, Richard Lynn recorded an increase in average IQ among the population of Japan.[30]

Intelligence

There is debate about whether the rise in IQ scores also corresponds to a rise in general intelligence, or only a rise in special skills related to taking IQ tests. Because children attend school longer now and have become much more familiar with the testing of school-related material, one might expect the greatest gains to occur on such school content-related tests as vocabulary, arithmetic or general information. Just the opposite is the case: abilities such as these have experienced relatively small gains and even occasional decreases over the years. Meta-analytic findings indicate that Flynn effects occur for tests assessing both fluid and crystallized abilities. For example, Dutch conscripts gained 21 points during only 30 years, or 7 points per decade, between 1952 and 1982.[20] This rise in IQ test scores is not wholly explained by an increase in general intelligence. Studies have shown that while test scores have improved over time, the improvement is not fully correlated with latent factors related to intelligence.[31] Other researchers argue that the IQ gains described by the Flynn effect are due in part to increasing intelligence, and in part to increases in test-specific skills.[32][33][34] One study suggested that the IQ gains reflected changes in modes of thinking that better reflected cognitive skills assessed by IQ tests rather than raw intelligence itself.[35]

Proposed explanations

Schooling and test familiarity

The duration of average schooling has increased steadily. However, a criticism of this explanation is that if (in the United States) older and younger subjects, with similar educational levels, are compared together, then the IQ gains appear almost undiminished in each group compared to when they are considered individually.[20]

Many studies find that children who do not attend school score drastically lower on the tests than their regularly attending peers. During the 1960s, when some Virginia counties closed their public schools to avoid racial integration, compensatory private schooling was available only for White children. On average, the scores of African-American children who received no formal education during that period decreased at a rate of about six IQ points per year.[20]

Another explanation is an increased familiarity of the general population with tests and testing. For example, children who take the very same IQ test a second time usually gain five or six points. However, this seems to set an upper limit on the effects of test sophistication. One problem with this explanation and others related to schooling is that in the US, the groups with greater test familiarity show smaller IQ increases.[20]

Early intervention programs have shown mixed results. Some preschool (ages 3–4) intervention programs like "Head Start" do not produce lasting changes of IQ, although they may confer other benefits.[which?] The "Abecedarian Early Intervention Project", an all-day program that provided various forms of environmental enrichment to children from infancy onward, showed IQ gains that did not diminish over time. The IQ gains in the experimental group compared to the control group was 4.4 points. These gains persisted until at least age 21.[36]

Citing a high correlation between rising literacy rates and gains in IQ, David Marks has argued that the Flynn effect is caused by changes in literacy rates.[37]

Nutrition

Improved nutrition is another possible explanation. Today's average adult from an industrialized nation is taller than a comparable adult of a century ago. That increase of stature, likely the result of general improvements in nutrition and health, has been at a rate of more than a centimeter per decade. Available data suggest that these gains have been accompanied by analogous increases in head size, and by an increase in the average size of the brain.[20][38] This argument had been thought to suffer the difficulty that groups who tend to be of smaller overall body size (e.g. women, or people of Asian ancestry) do not have lower average IQs.[25]

A 2005 study presented data supporting the nutrition hypothesis, which predicts that gains will occur predominantly at the low end of the IQ distribution, where nutritional deprivation is probably most severe.[26] An alternative interpretation of skewed IQ gains could be that improved education has been particularly important for this group.[25]

A century ago, nutritional deficiencies may have limited body and organ functionality, including skull volume. The first two years of life are a critical time for nutrition. The consequences of malnutrition can be irreversible and may include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.[39] On the other hand, Flynn has pointed to 20-point gains on Dutch military (Raven's type) IQ tests between 1952, 1962, 1972, and 1982. In 1962 he observed that Dutch 18-year-olds had a major nutritional handicap. They were either in the womb or were recently born, during the great Dutch famine of 1944—when German troops monopolized food and 18,000 people died of starvation.[40] Yet, concludes Flynn, "they do not show up even as a blip in the pattern of Dutch IQ gains. It is as if the famine had never occurred."[41][42] It appears that the effects of diet are gradual, taking effect over decades (affecting mother as well as the child) rather than a few months.[citation needed]

In support of the nutritional hypothesis, it is known that, in the United States, the average height before 1900 was about 10 cm (~4 inches) shorter than it is today.[43] Possibly related to the Flynn effect is a similar change of skull size and shape during the last 150 years. A Norwegian study found that height gains were strongly correlated with intelligence gains until the cessation of height gains in military conscript cohorts towards the end of the 1980s.[44] Both height and skull size increases probably result from a combination of phenotypic plasticity and genetic selection over this period.[45] With only five or six human generations in 150 years, time for natural selection has been very limited, suggesting that increased skeletal size resulting from changes in population phenotypes is more likely than recent genetic evolution.

It is well known that micronutrient deficiencies change the development of intelligence. For instance, one study has found that iodine deficiency causes a fall, on average, of 12 IQ points in China.[46]

Scientists James Feyrer, Dimitra Politi, and David N. Weil have found in the U.S. that the proliferation of iodized salt increased IQ by 15 points in some areas. Journalist Max Nisen has stated that with this type of salt becoming popular, that "the aggregate effect has been extremely positive."[47]

Daley et al. (2003) found a significant Flynn effect among children in rural Kenya, and concluded that nutrition was one of the hypothesized explanations that best explained their results (the others were parental literacy and family structure).[48]

Generally more stimulating environment

Still, another theory is that the general environment today is much more complex and stimulating. One of the most striking 20th-century changes in the human intellectual environment has come from the increase of exposure to many types of visual media. From pictures on the wall to movies to television to video games to computers, each successive generation has been exposed to richer optical displays than the one before and may have become more adept at visual analysis. This would explain why visual tests like the Raven's have shown the greatest increases. An increase only of particular forms of intelligence would explain why the Flynn effect has not caused a "cultural renaissance too great to be overlooked."[20]

In 2001, William Dickens and James Flynn presented a model for resolving several contradictory findings regarding IQ. They argue that the measure "heritability" includes both a direct effect of the genotype on IQ and also indirect effects such that the genotype changes the environment, thereby affecting IQ. That is, those with a greater IQ tend to seek stimulating environments that further increase IQ. These reciprocal effects result in gene environment correlation. The direct effect could initially have been very small, but feedback can create large differences in IQ. In their model, an environmental stimulus can have a very great effect on IQ, even for adults, but this effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues (the model could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition during early childhood, that may cause permanent effects). The Flynn effect can be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all people. The authors suggest that any program designed to increase IQ may produce long-term IQ gains if that program teaches children how to replicate the types of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ gains outside the program. To maximize lifetime IQ, the programs should also motivate them to continue searching for cognitively demanding experiences after they have left the program.[49][50]

Flynn, in his 2007 book What Is Intelligence?, further expanded on this theory. Environmental changes resulting from modernization—such as more intellectually demanding work, greater use of technology, and smaller families—have meant that a much larger proportion of people are more accustomed to manipulating abstract concepts such as hypotheses and categories than a century ago. Substantial portions of IQ tests deal with these abilities. Flynn gives, as an example, the question 'What do a dog and a rabbit have in common?' A modern respondent might say they are both mammals (an abstract, or a priori answer, which depends only on the meanings of the words dog and rabbit), whereas someone a century ago might have said that humans catch rabbits with dogs (a concrete, or a posteriori answer, which depended on what happened to be the case at that time).[51][52][better source needed]

Infectious diseases

Eppig, Fincher, and Thornhill (2011) conducted a study looking at different US states found that states with a higher prevalence of infectious diseases had lower average IQ. The effect remained after controlling for the effects of wealth and educational variation.[53]

Atheendar Venkataramani (2010) studied the effect of malaria on IQ in a sample of Mexicans. Malaria eradication during the birth year was associated with increases in IQ. It also increased the probability of employment in a skilled occupation. The author suggests that this may be one explanation for the Flynn effect and that this may be an important explanation for the link between national malaria burden and economic development.[54] A literature review of 44 papers states that cognitive abilities and school performance were shown to be impaired in sub-groups of patients (with either cerebral malaria or uncomplicated malaria) when compared with healthy controls. Studies comparing cognitive functions before and after treatment for acute malarial illness continued to show significantly impaired school performance and cognitive abilities even after recovery. Malaria prophylaxis was shown to improve cognitive function and school performance in clinical trials when compared to placebo groups.[55]

Heterosis

Heterosis, or hybrid vigor, associated with historical reductions of the levels of inbreeding, has been proposed by Michael Mingroni as an alternative explanation of the Flynn effect.[56][57] However, James Flynn has pointed out that even if everyone mated with a sibling in 1900, subsequent increases in heterosis would not be a sufficient explanation of the observed IQ gains.[58]

Reduction of lead in gasoline

One study found the drop in blood lead levels in the United States from the 1970s to 2007 correlated with a 4-5 point increase in IQ.[59]

Possible end of progression

Mean standing height and mean GA (both in z scores units+5) by year of testing, from Sundet et al. 2004 (figure 3)

Jon Martin Sundet and colleagues (2004) examined scores on intelligence tests given to Norwegian conscripts between the 1950s and 2002. They found that the increase of scores of general intelligence stopped after the mid-1990s and declined in numerical reasoning sub-tests.[44]

Teasdale and Owen (2005) examined the results of IQ tests given to Danish male conscripts. Between 1959 and 1979 the gains were 3 points per decade. Between 1979 and 1989 the increase approached 2 IQ points. Between 1989 and 1998 the gain was about 1.3 points. Between 1998 and 2004 IQ declined by about the same amount as it gained between 1989 and 1998. They speculate that "a contributing factor in this recent fall could be a simultaneous decline in proportions of students entering 3-year advanced-level school programs for 16–18-year-olds."[60] The same authors in a more comprehensive 2008 study, again on Danish male conscripts, found that there was a 1.5-point increase between 1988 and 1998, but a 1.5-point decrease between 1998 and 2003/2004.[61]

In Australia, the IQ of 6–12-year-olds (as measured by colored progressive matrices) has shown no increase from 1975 to 2003.[62]

In the United Kingdom, a study by Flynn (2009) himself found that tests carried out in 1980 and again in 2008 show that the IQ score of an average 14-year-old dropped by more than two points over the period. For the upper half of the results, the performance was even worse. Average IQ scores declined by six points. However, children aged between five and 10 saw their IQs increase by up to half a point a year over the three decades. Flynn argues that the abnormal drop in British teenage IQ could be due to youth culture having "stagnated" or even dumbed down.[63]

Bratsberg & Rogeberg (2018) present evidence that the Flynn effect in Norway has reversed between the years 1962–1991, and that both the original rise in mean IQ scores and their subsequent decline within this period can be observed within families consisting of native-born parents and their children, indicating that environmental factors were the likely cause for these changes. Because IQ data was only available for male Norwegians, who were subject to military conscription, years of schooling were used as an approximation for female IQ to support this conclusion.[8]

One possible explanation of a worldwide decline in intelligence is an increase in air pollution; coal burning emits mercury, and intelligence has continued to climb in areas, like the southern United States, where coal burning has declined.[64][65]

Winter et al. (2024) when comparing two WAIS-5 validity studies found a reduced Flynn effect of an increase of 1.2 IQ points per decade rather than the expected 3 IQ point increase per decade. The authors identified various novel factors including social media dependency and the COVID-19 pandemic which may have contributed to a reduced Flynn effect.[66]

IQ group differences

Gains in IQ that different world regions have made since the first year for which data is available for a particular region.

If the Flynn effect has ended in developed nations but continues in less developed ones, this would tend to diminish national differences in IQ scores.[61]

Also, if the Flynn effect has ended for the majority in developed nations, it may still continue for minorities, especially for groups like immigrants where many may have received poor nutrition during early childhood or have had other disadvantages. A study in the Netherlands found that children of non-Western immigrants had improvements for g, educational achievements, and work proficiency compared to their parents, although there were still remaining differences compared to ethnic Dutch.[33]

In the United States, the IQ gap between black and white people was gradually closing over the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults.[67] Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,[68] a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished.[69] Reviews by Flynn and Dickens,[70] Nicholas Mackintosh,[71][page needed] and Nisbett et al.[72][73] all concluded that the gradual closing of the gap was a real phenomenon.

Flynn has commented that he never claimed that the Flynn effect has the same causes as observed differences in average IQ test performance between blacks and whites, but that it shows that environmental factors can create IQ differences of a magnitude similar to that gap.[74] Wicherts et al. had previously suggested a similar interpretation in a 2004 paper.[75] Flynn also argued that his findings undermine the so-called Spearman's hypothesis, which hypothesized that differences in g factor are the major driver of the blacks-whites IQ gap.[76]

See also

References

  1. ^ Trahan, Lisa H.; Stuebing, Karla K.; Fletcher, Jack M.; Hiscock, Merrill (2014). "The Flynn effect: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1332–1360. doi:10.1037/a0037173. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 4152423. PMID 24979188.
  2. ^ a b Baker, David P.; Eslinger, Paul J.; Benavides, Martin; Peters, Ellen; Dieckmann, Nathan F.; Leon, Juan (March 2015). "The cognitive impact of the education revolution: A possible cause of the Flynn Effect on population IQ". Intelligence. 49: 144–58. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.01.003. ISSN 0160-2896.
  3. ^ Flynn, James R. (March 2009). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938–2008". Economics and Human Biology. 7 (1): 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009. ISSN 1873-6130. PMID 19251490.
  4. ^ a b Rönnlund M, Nilsson LG (September 2009). "Flynn effects on sub-factors of episodic and semantic memory: parallel gains over time and the same set of determining factors". Neuropsychologia. 47 (11): 2174–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.007. PMID 19056409. S2CID 15706086.
  5. ^ Al-Shahomee; et al. (2018). "An increase of intelligence in Libya from 2008 to 2017". Personality and Individual Differences. 130: 147–149. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.004. S2CID 149095461.
  6. ^ Teasdale, Thomas W; Owen, David R (2005). "A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (4): 837–43. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.029.
  7. ^ Pietschnig, Jakob; Gittler, Georg (2015). "A reversal of the Flynn effect for spatial perception in German-speaking countries: Evidence from a cross-temporal IRT-based meta-analysis (1977–2014)". Intelligence. 53: 145–53. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.10.004.
  8. ^ a b Bratsberg, Bernt; Rogeberg, Ole (June 6, 2018). "Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (26): 6674–78. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.6674B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1718793115. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 6042097. PMID 29891660.
  9. ^ Gonthier, Corentin; Grégoire, Jacques; Besançon, Maud (January 2021). "No negative Flynn effect in France: Why variations of intelligence should not be assessed using tests based on cultural knowledge". Intelligence. 84: 101512. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2020.101512. S2CID 230538271.
  10. ^ Trahan, Lisa H.; Stuebing, Karla K.; Fletcher, Jack M.; Hiscock, Merrill (2014). "The Flynn effect: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1332–1360. doi:10.1037/a0037173. PMC 4152423. PMID 24979188.
  11. ^ Pietschnig, Jakob; Voracek, Martin (May 2015). "One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (3): 282–306. doi:10.1177/1745691615577701. PMID 25987509. S2CID 12604392.
  12. ^ Wongupparaj, Peera; Kumari, Veena; Morris, Robin G. (March 2015). "A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Raven's Progressive Matrices: Age groups and developing versus developed countries". Intelligence. 49: 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.008.
  13. ^ Flynn, James R. (2009). What Is Intelligence: Beyond the Flynn Effect (expanded paperback ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–2. ISBN 978-0-521-74147-7. The 'Flynn effect' is the name that has become attached to an exciting development, namely, that the twentieth century saw massive IQ gains from one generation to another. To forestall a diagnosis of megalomania, the label was coined by Herrnstein and Murray, the authors of The Bell Curve, and not by myself.
  14. ^ Shalizi, Cosma (April 27, 2009). "The Domestication of the Savage Mind". University of Michigan (Review). Archived from the original on July 19, 2012. Retrieved August 13, 2010.
  15. ^ Herrnstein, Richard J.; Murray, Charles (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: The Free Press. p. 307. ISBN 0-02-914673-9.
  16. ^ Haig, Brian D. (July 1, 2013). "Detecting Psychological Phenomena: Taking Bottom-Up Research Seriously". The American Journal of Psychology. 126 (2): 135–153. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.2.0135. ISSN 0002-9556. PMID 23858950.
  17. ^ Flynn, James R. (2011), "Secular Changes in Intelligence" (PDF), The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, pp. 647–665, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511977244.033, ISBN 978-0-511-97724-4, retrieved March 27, 2023
  18. ^ Tuddenham, R. D. (1948). "Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II". The American Psychologist. 3 (2): 54–56. doi:10.1037/h0054962. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 18911933.
  19. ^ Fletcher, Richard B.; Hattie, John (2011). Intelligence and Intelligence Testing. Taylor & Francis. p. 26. ISBN 978-1-136-82321-3. Retrieved August 31, 2013. Indeed, this effect, now called the 'Flynn effect', is well established. Nations, almost without exception, have shown gains of about 20 IQ points per generation (30 years). These gains are highest for IQ tests that are most related to reasoning and the capacity to figure out novel problems (this is often called 'fluid intelligence', see Chapter 5); and least related to knowledge, which arises from better educational opportunity, a history of persistence and good motivation for learning (this is often called 'crystallized intelligence', see Chapter 5).
    • Freeman, Joan (2010). Gifted Lives: What Happens when Gifted Children Grow Up. London: Routledge. pp. 290–91. ISBN 978-0-415-47009-4. A strange new phenomenon has been growing since about 1950, called the 'Flynn Effect' after Professor James Flynn of the University of Otago, New Zealand. In his book What is Intelligence ?, Flynn describes a year-on-year rise in measured intelligence, about three IQ points a decade.
    • Barbieri, Annalisa (October 8, 2010). "Young, gifted and likely to suffer for it". The Guardian. Archived from the original on November 11, 2020. Retrieved December 14, 2016.
    • Urbina, Susana (2004). Essentials of Psychological Testing. John Wiley & Sons. p. 103. ISBN 978-0-471-41978-5. A puzzling longitudinal trend in the opposite direction, known as the 'Flynn effect', has been well documented in successive revisions of major intelligence tests (like the S-B and the Wechsler scales) that invariably involve the administration of both the old and new versions to a segment of the newer standardization sample, for comparative purposes. Data from revisions of various intelligence tests in the United States as well as in other countries—extensively analyzed by J.R. Flynn (1984, 1987)—show a pronounced, long-term upward trend in the level of performance required to obtain any given IQ score. The Flynn effect presumably reflects population gains over time in the kinds of cognitive performance that intelligence tests sample. Wasserman, John D. (2012). "Chapter 18: Assessment of Intellectual Functioning". In Weiner, Irving B.; Graham, John R.; Naglieri, Jack A. (eds.). Handbook of Psychology. Vol. 10: Assessment Psychology. John Wiley & Sons. p. 486. ISBN 978-0-470-89127-8. Both definitions also specify that the intellectual functioning criterion for a diagnosis of intellectual disability is approximately 2 SDs or more below the normative mean, but factors such as test score statistical error (standard error of measurement), test fairness, normative expectations for the population of interest, the Flynn effect, and practice effects from previous testing need to be considered before arriving at any diagnosis.
    • Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (2011). Personality and Individual Differences. Wiley. p. 221. ISBN 978-1-4051-9927-8. Flynn effect The finding by sociologist James Flynn that there are generational increases in IQ across nations.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g h i Neisser U (1997). "Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests". American Scientist. 85 (5): 440–47. Bibcode:1997AmSci..85..440N. Archived from the original on November 4, 2016.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  21. ^ Joseph Lee Rodgers (2014). "http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/rodgers2014.pdf Intelligence Are birth order effects on intelligence really Flynn Effects? Reinterpreting Belmont and Marolla 40 years later" (PDF). Intelligence 42: 128-133. "No within-family data exist that document an increase in intelligence over birth order, suggesting that its source derives from outside the family and will only manifest in data and analyses that account for between-family variance (such as cross-sectional data)." (p. 130)
  22. ^ Trahan, LH; Stuebing, KK; Fletcher, JM; Hiscock, M (September 2014). "The Flynn effect: a meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1332–60. doi:10.1037/a0037173. PMC 4152423. PMID 24979188.
  23. ^ a b Jakob Pietschnig; Martin Voracek (May 1, 2015). "One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (3): 282–306. doi:10.1177/1745691615577701. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 25987509. S2CID 12604392.
  24. ^ Raven John (2000). "The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Change and Stability over Culture and Time" (PDF). Cognitive Psychology. 41 (1): 1–48. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735. PMID 10945921. S2CID 26363133. Archived (PDF) from the original on April 28, 2019. Retrieved July 9, 2011.
  25. ^ a b c Teasdale, T. (1989). "Continuing secular increases in intelligence and a stable prevalence of high intelligence levels". Intelligence. 13 (3): 255–62. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(89)90021-4.
  26. ^ a b Colom, R.; Lluis-Font, J.M. & Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2005). "The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: Supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis" (PDF). Intelligence. 33 (1): 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010. Archived (PDF) from the original on August 13, 2012. Retrieved October 4, 2012.
  27. ^ Tucker, Ian (September 22, 2012). "James Flynn: IQ may go up as well as down". The Guardian. ISSN 0261-3077. Archived from the original on January 21, 2019. Retrieved January 21, 2019.
  28. ^ Calhoun, Daniel (1973). The Intelligence of a People. Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-04619-8.
  29. ^ Thorndike, Robert L. (1975). "Mr. Binet's Test 70 Years Later". Educational Researcher. 4 (5): 3–7. doi:10.3102/0013189X004005003. ISSN 0013-189X. JSTOR 1174855. S2CID 145355731.
  30. ^ Lynn, Richard (May 1982). "IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity". Nature. 297 (5863): 222–223. Bibcode:1982Natur.297..222L. doi:10.1038/297222a0. ISSN 1476-4687. S2CID 4331657.
  31. ^ Must O, Must A, Raudik V (2003). "The secular rise in IQs: In Estonia, the Flynn effect is not a Jensen effect" (PDF). Intelligence. 31 (5): 461–71. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00013-8. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 11, 2017. Retrieved September 13, 2011.
  32. ^ Wicherts, J.M.; Dolan, C.V.; Hessen, D.J.; Oosterveld, P.; Baal, G.C.M. van; Boomsma, D.I. & Span, M.M. (2004). "Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect" (PDF). Intelligence. 32 (5): 509537. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.207.4350. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002. Archived from the original (PDF) on May 29, 2005. The overall conclusion of the present paper is that factorial invariance with respect to cohorts is not tenable . . . . The fact that the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure, should not sit well with explanations that appeal solely to changes at the level of the latent variables.
  33. ^ a b Te Nijenhuis J, De Jong MJ, Evers A, Van Der Flier H (2004). "Are cognitive differences between immigrant and majority groups diminishing?" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 18 (5): 405–34. doi:10.1002/per.511. S2CID 4806581. Archived (PDF) from the original on February 21, 2021. Retrieved November 10, 2019.
  34. ^ Colom R, Garcia-Lopez O (2003). "Secular Gains in Fluid Intelligence: Evidence from the Culture-Fair Intelligence Test". Journal of Biosocial Science. 35 (1): 33–39. doi:10.1017/S0021932003000336. PMID 12537154. S2CID 24493926.
  35. ^ Flynn, James Robert; Te Nijenhuis, Jan; Metzen, Daniel (May–June 2014). "The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses". Intelligence. 44: 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009. Retrieved July 2, 2023.
  36. ^ Plomin R.; DeFries J.C.; Craig I.W.; McGuffin P. (2003). Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era (4th ed.).
  37. ^ Marks, David Francis (June 1, 2010). "IQ Variations across Time, Race, and Nationality: An Artifact of Differences in Literacy Skills". Psychological Reports. 106 (3): 643–664. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664. ISSN 0033-2941. PMID 20712152 – via Sage Publishing.
  38. ^ Jantz, R.; Meadows Jantz, L. (2000). "Secular change in craniofacial morphology". American Journal of Human Biology. 12 (3): 327–38. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(200005/06)12:3<327::AID-AJHB3>3.0.CO;2-1. PMID 11534023. S2CID 22059721.
  39. ^ "The Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition". 2008. Archived from the original on July 17, 2011. Retrieved February 11, 2011.
  40. ^ C. Banning (1946). "Food Shortage and Public Health, First Half of 1945". Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science Vol. 245, The Netherlands during German Occupation (May 1946), pp. 93–110
  41. ^ Flynn J.R. (1987). "Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure". Psychological Bulletin. 101 (2): 171–91. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171.
  42. ^ Flynn, James R. (2009). What Is Intelligence? (p. 103). Cambridge University Press. Kindle Edition.
  43. ^ Samaras, Thomas T.; Elrick, Harold (May 2002). "Group Height, body size, and longevity: is smaller better for the human body?". West J Med. 176 (3): 206–08. doi:10.1136/ewjm.176.3.206. PMC 1071721. PMID 12016250.
  44. ^ a b Sundet, J.; Barlaug, D.; Torjussen, T. (2004). "The end of the Flynn effect?: A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century". Intelligence. 32 (4): 349–62. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.004.
  45. ^ Jantz RL, Meadows Jantz L (May 2000). "Secular change in craniofacial morphology". Am. J. Hum. Biol. 12 (3): 327–38. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(200005/06)12:3<327::AID-AJHB3>3.0.CO;2-1. PMID 11534023. S2CID 22059721.
    Jantz RL (July 2001). "Cranial change in Americans: 1850–1975". J. Forensic Sci. 46 (4): 784–87. doi:10.1520/JFS15047J. PMID 11451056.
  46. ^ Qian M; Wang D; Watkins WE; et al. (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706.
  47. ^ Nisen, Max (July 22, 2013). "How Adding Iodine To Salt Resulted In A Decade's Worth Of IQ Gains For The United States". Business Insider. Retrieved January 23, 2023.
  48. ^ Daley, TC; Whaley, SE; Sigman, MD; Espinosa, MP; Neumann, C (May 2003). "IQ on the rise: the Flynn effect in rural Kenyan children". Psychological Science. 14 (3): 215–19. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02434. PMID 12741743. S2CID 12315212.
  49. ^ Dickens WT, Flynn JR (2001). "Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved" (PDF). Psychological Review. 108 (2): 346–369. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.139.2436. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346. PMID 11381833.
  50. ^ Dickens WT, Flynn JR (2002). "The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved" (PDF). Psychological Review. 109 (4): 764–71. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.764. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 19, 2007.
  51. ^ Flynn, James R. (August 2012). What Is Intelligence? Beyond the Flynn Effect. Cambridge University Press. pp. 24–29. ISBN 9780511605253.
  52. ^ Gladwell, Malcolm (December 10, 2007). "None of the Above". The New Yorker. Retrieved July 6, 2024.
  53. ^ Eppig C, Fincher CL, Thornhill R (2011). "Parasite prevalence and the distribution of intelligence among the states of the USA". Intelligence. 39 (2–3): 155–60. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.02.008.
  54. ^ Venkataramani A (September 18, 2010). "Early Life Exposure to Malaria and Cognition and Skills in Adulthood: Evidence from Mexico". SSRN 1679164.
  55. ^ Fernando SD, Rodrigo C, Rajapakse S (2010). "The 'hidden' burden of malaria: cognitive impairment following infection". Malar. J. 9 (1): 366. doi:10.1186/1475-2875-9-366. PMC 3018393. PMID 21171998.
  56. ^ Mingroni, M. A. (2007). "Resolving the IQ paradox: Heterosis as a cause of the Flynn effect and other trends" (PDF). Psychological Review, 114(3), 806–829. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.806
  57. ^ Mingroni, M.A. (2004). "The secular rise in IQ: Giving heterosis a second look" (PDF). Intelligence, 32, 65-83.
  58. ^ Mackintosh, N.J. (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 291.
  59. ^ Kaufman, Alan S.; Zhou, Xiaobin; Reynolds, Matthew R.; Kaufman, Nadeen L.; Green, Garo P.; Weisse, Lawrence G. (July 2014). "The possible societal impact of the decrease in U.S. blood lead levels on adult IQ". Environmental Research. 132: 413–420. Bibcode:2014ER....132..413K. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2014.04.015. PMID 24853978.
  60. ^ Teasdale, Thomas W.; Owen, David R. (2005). "A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (4): 837–43. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.029.
  61. ^ a b Teasdale TW, Owen DR (2008). "Secular declines in cognitive test scores: A reversal of the Flynn Effect" (PDF). Intelligence. 36 (2): 121–26. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.01.007. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 15, 2012. Retrieved April 18, 2010.
  62. ^ Cotton, S.M.; Kiely, P.M.; Crewther, D.P.; Thomson, B.; Laycock, R.; Crewther, S.G. (2005). "A normative and reliability study for the Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices for primary school aged children in Australia". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (3): 647–60. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.015.
  63. ^ Flynn, J.R. (2009). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938–2008". Economics & Human Biology. 7 (1): 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009. PMID 19251490.
  64. ^ Pesovski, Ivica; Kulakov, Andrea; Trajkovikj, Vladimir (2022). "Differences in cognitive ability assessment results between Millennial and Generation Z cohorts". The 19th International Conference on Informatics and Information Technologies – CIIT 2022.
  65. ^ Bratsberg, Bernt; Rogeberg, Ole (June 26, 2018). "Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (26): 6674–6678. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.6674B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1718793115. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 6042097. PMID 29891660.
  66. ^ Winter, Emily L.; Trudel, Sierra M.; Kaufman, Alan S. (November 15, 2024). "Wait, Where's the Flynn Effect on the WAIS-5?". Journal of Intelligence. 12 (11): 118. doi:10.3390/jintelligence12110118. ISSN 2079-3200. PMC 11595985.
  67. ^ Vincent, Ken R. (March 1991). "Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference?". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 47 (2): 266–270. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s. PMID 2030133.
  68. ^ Dickens & Flynn 2006.
  69. ^ Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association
  70. ^ Dickens, William T; Flynn, James R (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples" (PDF). Psychological Science. 17 (10): 913–20. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.186.2540. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x. PMID 17100793. S2CID 6593169. Archived from the original (PDF) on October 9, 2009.
  71. ^ Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence (second ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-958559-5.
  72. ^ Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012a). "Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments" (PDF). American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22233090. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 8, 2020. Retrieved July 22, 2013.
  73. ^ Willingham, Dan (May 10, 2012). "The latest on intelligence". Daniel Willingham—Science & Education.
  74. ^ Flynn, J.R. (2010). "The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate". Intelligence. 38 (4): 363–66. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001.
  75. ^ Wicherts, Jelte M.; Dolan, Conor V.; Hessen, David J.; Oosterveld, Paul; van Baal, G. Caroline M.; Boomsma, Dorret I.; Span, Mark M. (2004). "Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect". Intelligence. 32 (5): 509–537. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002. It appears therefore that the nature of the Flynn effect is qualitatively different from the nature of B–W differences in the United States. Each comparison of groups should be investigated separately. IQ gaps between cohorts do not teach us anything about IQ gaps between contemporary groups, except that each IQ gap should not be confused with real (i.e., latent) differences in intelligence.
  76. ^ Flynn, J.R. (1999). "Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time" (PDF). American Psychologist. 54: 5–9. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5. Archived (PDF) from the original on June 25, 2010. Retrieved October 26, 2017.

Further reading

Read other articles:

Цивилизация Древнего мира • ЕвропаДревний Рим Амфитеатр Флавиев (Колизей) в Риме  • экзотопонимы — Дацинь Время 753 год до н. э. — 476 год  • периодизация — Царский Рим (754/753—510/509 до н. э.)  — Ранняя Римская республика (509—265 гг. до н. э.)  — Поздняя Римск�...

 

 

Current United States federal appellate court United States Court of Appeals for the First Circuit(1st Cir.)LocationJohn Joseph Moakley U.S. Courthouse(Boston, Massachusetts)Appeals fromDistrict of MaineDistrict of MassachusettsDistrict of New HampshireDistrict of Puerto RicoDistrict of Rhode IslandEstablishedJune 16, 1891Judges6Circuit JusticeKetanji Brown JacksonChief JudgeDavid J. Barronwww.ca1.uscourts.gov The United States Court of Appeals for the First Circuit (in case c...

 

 

Carl XVI Gustaf Pour les articles homonymes, voir Charles, prince de Suède. Charles XVI Gustave(sv) Carl XVI Gustaf Le roi Charles XVI Gustave en 2018. Titre Roi de Suède En fonction depuis le 15 septembre 1973(50 ans, 6 mois et 22 jours) Couronnement 19 septembre 1973 Premier ministre Olof Palme Thorbjörn Fälldin Ola Ullsten Thorbjörn Fälldin Olof Palme Ingvar Carlsson Carl Bildt Ingvar Carlsson Göran Persson Fredrik Reinfeldt Stefan Löfven Magdalena AnderssonUlf Kris...

Orthodox, Chabad-Lubavitch Hasidic russian rabbi This biography of a living person needs additional citations for verification. Please help by adding reliable sources. Contentious material about living persons that is unsourced or poorly sourced must be removed immediately from the article and its talk page, especially if potentially libelous.Find sources: Berel Lazar – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (October 2023) (Learn how and when to r...

 

 

Arknights Logo Arknights versi globalPublikasiCHN: 1 Mei 2019JP/KR: 15 Januari 2020WW: 16 Januari 2020TW: 29 Juni 2020GenreBermain peran, pertahanan menaraKarakteristik teknisSistem operasiAndroid dan iOS PlatformiOS dan Android MesinUnity3DFormatdistribusi digital dan unduhan digital Metode inputlayar sentuh Format kode Daftar 30 Informasi pengembangPengembangHypergryphPenyuntingHyperGryph (en) (Tiongkok Daratan)Yostar (en) (Jepang, Korea Selatan, anglosfer)hasuhasu (en) (Two sides, three pl...

 

 

追晉陸軍二級上將趙家驤將軍个人资料出生1910年 大清河南省衛輝府汲縣逝世1958年8月23日(1958歲—08—23)(47—48歲) † 中華民國福建省金門縣国籍 中華民國政党 中國國民黨获奖 青天白日勳章(追贈)军事背景效忠 中華民國服役 國民革命軍 中華民國陸軍服役时间1924年-1958年军衔 二級上將 (追晉)部队四十七師指挥東北剿匪總司令部參謀長陸軍�...

Open source software project OSVR Hacker Developer Kit 1.4NameOSVR Hacker Developer Kit (HDK)TypeVirtual reality headsetManufacturerRazerDesigned bySensics and RazerDisplay TechnologyOLEDResolution1200x1080 per eye[1]Head Tracking6DOF (3-axis rotational tracking + 3-axis positional tracking)PlatformsMicrosoft Windows, macOS, Android and LinuxConnection2x USB 3.0, HDMI 1.4, Audio outWebsiteOfficial website Open Source Virtual Reality (OSVR) was an open-source software project that aime...

 

 

  「俄亥俄」重定向至此。关于其他用法,请见「俄亥俄 (消歧义)」。 俄亥俄州 美國联邦州State of Ohio 州旗州徽綽號:七葉果之州地图中高亮部分为俄亥俄州坐标:38°27'N-41°58'N, 80°32'W-84°49'W国家 美國加入聯邦1803年3月1日,在1953年8月7日追溯頒定(第17个加入联邦)首府哥倫布(及最大城市)政府 • 州长(英语:List of Governors of {{{Name}}}]]) •&...

 

 

هنودمعلومات عامةنسبة التسمية الهند التعداد الكليالتعداد قرابة 1.21 مليار[1][2]تعداد الهند عام 2011ق. 1.32 مليار[3]تقديرات عام 2017ق. 30.8 مليون[4]مناطق الوجود المميزةبلد الأصل الهند البلد الهند  الهند نيبال 4,000,000[5] الولايات المتحدة 3,982,398[6] الإمار...

Soyuz TMA-20MSoyuz TMA-20M mendekati ISSOperatorRoscosmosCOSPAR ID2016-018ASATCAT no.41391Durasi misi172 hari 3 jam 47 menit Properti wahanaJenis wahana antariksaSoyuz-TMA-M 11F747 No.720[1]ProdusenRKK Energia AwakJumlah awak3AwakAleksey OvchininOleg SkripochkaJeffrey N. WilliamsTanda panggilBurlak Awal misiTanggal luncur18 Maret 2016 21:26:38 UTCRoket peluncurSoyuz-FGTempat peluncuranBaikonur 1/5 Akhir MisiTanggal mendarat7 September 201601:13 UTCTempat pendaratanKazakhstan Parameter...

 

 

本條目存在以下問題,請協助改善本條目或在討論頁針對議題發表看法。 此條目需要編修,以確保文法、用詞、语气、格式、標點等使用恰当。 (2013年8月6日)請按照校對指引,幫助编辑這個條目。(幫助、討論) 此條目剧情、虛構用語或人物介紹过长过细,需清理无关故事主轴的细节、用語和角色介紹。 (2020年10月6日)劇情、用語和人物介紹都只是用於了解故事主軸,輔助�...

 

 

Sporting event delegationEast Timor at the2022 World Athletics ChampionshipsFlag of Timor-LesteWA codeTLSin Eugene, United States15 July 2022 (2022-07-15) – 24 July 2022 (2022-07-24)Competitors1 (1 man)Medals Gold 0 Silver 0 Bronze 0 Total 0 World Athletics Championships appearances (overview)201120132015–2017201920222023← 2019 2023 → Timor-Leste or East Timor competed at the 2022 World Athletics Championships in Eugene, United States, ...

مناورة فالين من أنواع اختبار تشخيصي  [لغات أخرى]‏  تعديل مصدري - تعديل   قطع مستعرض عبر الرسغ، والأصابع (العصب المتوسط هو النقطة الصفراء بالقرب من المركز، النفق الرسغي غير موضح بالرسم، ولكن الهيكل الدائري المحيط بالعصب المتوسط مرئي). مناورة فالين (بالإنجليزية: P...

 

 

Controversies about who gets to be a member of the Boy Scouts of America The Boy Scouts of America (BSA), one of the largest private youth organizations in the United States, has policies which prohibit those who are not willing to subscribe to the BSA's Declaration of Religious Principle, which has been interpreted by some as banning atheists,[1] and, until January 2014, prohibited all known or avowed homosexuals, from membership in its Scouting program.[2] The ban on adults ...

 

 

Deaf sign language of Peru Sivia Sign LanguageNative toPeruRegionSiviaNative speakers12 native speakers (2015–2016)[1]15–18 proficient, plus additional learnersLanguage familyvillage signLanguage codesISO 639-3lsvGlottologsivi1235 Sivia Sign Language is the deaf sign language of the Quechua town of Sivia in Peru. It is not related to Peruvian Sign Language.[2] The first generation consists of a deaf woman born in 1972, her deaf younger sister born in 1984, and a ...

Questa voce sull'argomento tecnologia è solo un abbozzo. Contribuisci a migliorarla secondo le convenzioni di Wikipedia. Segui i suggerimenti del progetto di riferimento. Moderno solarium Il solarium (latino, derivato di sol-solis, il sole; propr. luogo soleggiato) è un impianto tecnico che si serve dei raggi ultravioletti, per lo più di tipo A (UV-A), per irradiare il corpo umano a scopi terapeutici o per indurre un'abbronzatura artificiale. Indice 1 Descrizione 1.1 Tipi 1.2 Limiti ...

 

 

Italy's national women's volleyball team ItalyAssociationFIPAVConfederationCEVHead coachJulio VelascoFIVB ranking1 (as of 1 July 2024)Uniforms Home Away Summer OlympicsAppearances6 (First in 2000)Best result5th (2004, 2008, 2012)World ChampionshipAppearances11 (First in 1978)Best result (2002)World CupAppearances4 (First in 1999)Best result (2007, 2011)European ChampionshipAppearances26 (First in 1951)Best result (2007, 2009, 2021)federvolley.it Honours Event 1st 2nd 3rd World Championship 1 ...

 

 

Pour les articles homonymes, voir Endeavour. HMS Endeavour Réplique de l’Endeavour à Cooktown. Autres noms Earl of Pembroke ; Lord Sandwich Type Bâtiment hydrographique Fonction Transport de charbon puis exploration (original) ; bateau-musée et navire-école (réplique) Gréement 3 mâts-carré Histoire Chantier naval Nord de l'Angleterre (original) ; Mews Road, Fremantle en Australie (réplique) Quille posée 1988 (réplique) Lancement Date de construction du navir...

Ne doit pas être confondu avec l'église Saint-Just, qui existe encore. Pour les articles homonymes, voir Saint-Just. Basilique Saint-Just Présentation Culte Catholique Type Basilique (détruite) Rattachement Archidiocèse de Lyon Protection Notice no PA00117985, sur la plateforme ouverte du patrimoine, base Mérimée, ministère français de la Culture Géographie Pays France Ville Lyon Coordonnées 45° 45′ 20″ nord, 4° 49′ 03″ est Géolocalisa...

 

 

Not to be confused with Fish Bulletin, published by the California Department of Fish and Game. Academic journalFishery BulletinDisciplineZoologyLanguageEnglishEdited byRichard D. BrodeurPublication detailsFormer name(s)Bulletin of the United States Fish Commission, Bulletin of the Bureau of Fisheries, Bulletin of the United States Fish Commission, Fishery Bulletin of the Fish and Wildlife ServiceHistory1881–presentPublisherNational Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (Uni...