Episodic memory is the memory of everyday events (such as times, location geography, associated emotions, and other contextual information) that can be explicitly stated or conjured. It is the collection of past personal experiences that occurred at particular times and places; for example, the party on one's 7th birthday.[1] Along with semantic memory, it comprises the category of explicit memory, one of the two major divisions of long-term memory (the other being implicit memory).[2]
The term "episodic memory" was coined by Endel Tulving in 1972, referring to the distinction between knowing and remembering: knowing is factual recollection (semantic) whereas remembering is a feeling that is located in the past (episodic).[3]
One of the main components of episodic memory is the process of recollection, which elicits the retrieval of contextual information pertaining to a specific event or experience that has occurred. Tulving seminally defined three key properties of episodic memory recollection as:
Autonoetic consciousness, a special kind of consciousness that accompanies the act of remembering, which enables an individual to be aware of the self in a subjective time
Aside from Tulving, others named additional aspects of recollection, including visual imagery, narrative structure, retrieval of semantic information and feelings of familiarity.[4]
Events that are recorded into episodic memory may trigger episodic learning, i.e. a change in behavior that occurs as a result of an event,[5][6] such as a fear of dogs after being bitten by a dog.
Nine properties
There are essentially nine properties of episodic memory that collectively distinguish it from other types of memory. Other types of memory may exhibit a few of these properties, but only episodic memory has all nine:[7]
Contain summary records of sensory-perceptual-conceptual-affective processing.
Retain patterns of activation/inhibition over long periods.
Often represented in the form of (visual) images.
They always have a perspective (field or observer).
Represent short time slices of experience.
They are represented on a temporal dimension roughly in order of occurrence.
They are subject to rapid forgetting.
They make autobiographical remembering specific.
They are recollectively experienced when accessed.
Cognitive neuroscience
The formation of new episodic memories requires the medial temporal lobe, a structure that includes the hippocampus. Without the medial temporal lobe, one is able to form new procedural memories (such as playing the piano) but cannot remember the events during which they happened (See the hippocampus and memory).
The prefrontal cortex (and in particular the right hemisphere) is also involved in the formation of new episodic memories (also known as episodic encoding). Patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex can learn new information, but tend to do so in a disordered fashion. For example, they might show normal recognition of an object they had seen in the past, but fail to recollect when or where it had been viewed.[8] Some researchers believe that the prefrontal cortex helps organize information for more efficient storage, drawing upon its role in executive function. Others believe that the prefrontal cortex underlies semantic strategies which enhance encoding, such as thinking about the meaning of the study material or rehearsing it in working memory.[9]
Other work has shown that portions of the inferior parietal lobe play a role in episodic memory, potentially acting as an accumulator to support the subjective feeling that something is "old", or perhaps supporting mental imagery which allows you a sense of the vividness of memories. Indeed, bilateral damage to the inferior parietal lobe results in episodic memory that is largely intact, however it lacks details [10] and lesion patients report low levels of confidence in their memories.[11]
Researchers do not agree about how long episodic memories are stored in the hippocampus. Some researchers believe that episodic memories always rely on the hippocampus. Others believe the hippocampus only stores episodic memories for a short time, after which the memories are consolidated to the neocortex. The latter view is strengthened by recent evidence that neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus may ease the removal of old memories and increase the efficiency of forming new memories.[12]
Relationship to semantic memory
Endel Tulving originally described episodic memory as a record of a person's experience that held temporally dated information and spatio-temporal relations.[13] A feature of episodic memory that Tulving later elaborates on is that it allows an agent to imagine traveling back in time.[14] A current situation may cue retrieval of a previous episode, so that context that colours the previous episode is experienced at the immediate moment. The agent is provided with a means of associating previous feelings with current situations. Semantic memory, on the other hand, is a structured record of facts, concepts, and skills that we have acquired. Semantic information is derived from accumulated episodic memory. Episodic memory can be thought of as a "map" that ties together items in semantic memory. For example, all encounters with how a "dog" looks and sounds will make up the semantic representation of that word. All episodic memories concerning a dog will then reference this single semantic representation of "dog" and, likewise, all new experiences with the dog will modify the single semantic representation of that dog.
Together, semantic and episodic memory make up our declarative memory.[15] They each represent different parts of context to form a complete picture. As such, something that affects episodic memory can also affect semantic memory. For example, anterograde amnesia, from damage of the medial temporal lobe, is an impairment of declarative memory that affects both episodic and semantic memory operations.[16] Originally, Tulving proposed that episodic and semantic memory were separate systems that competed with each other in retrieval. However, this theory was rejected when Howard and Kahana completed experiments on latent semantic analysis (LSA) that supported the opposite. Instead of an increase in semantic similarity when there was a decrease in the strength of temporal associations, the two worked together so semantic cues on retrieval were strongest when episodic cues were strong as well.[17]
Episodic memory emerges at approximately 3 to 4 years of age.[18] Activation of specific brain areas (mostly the hippocampus) seems to be different between younger (aged 23–39) and older people (aged 67–80) upon episodic memory retrieval.[19] Older people tend to activate both their left and right hippocampus, while younger people activate only the left one.
Relationship to emotion
The relationship between emotion and memory is complex, but generally, emotion tends to increase the likelihood that an event will be remembered later and that it will be remembered vividly. Flashbulb memory is one example of this. Flashbulb memory is event-specific, which consists of depictions of personal experiences. For example, saying "I remember seeing Grandma smile when I gave her the present", or remembering the detailed events of the tragedy of 9/11. This idea of flashbulb memory was proposed by R. Brown and Kulik (1977), in which they stated that this idea revolves around remembering an event or unexpected circumstance due to emotional arousal. They referred to this memory as "photographic vividness". However, whether the vividness of the flashbulb memory is due to a virtual "flash" that occurs because of the emotional experience has been hotly contested. Flashbulb memories may occur because of our propensity to rehearse and retell those highly emotional events, which strengthens the memory. R. Brown and Kulik represented that these memories contain information that falls under the categories: place, ongoing activity, informant, own affect, and aftermath. Flashbulb memory is usually perceived as highly accurate and consistent over time and are presented with great confidence, even if sometimes they are inaccurate. Authors Brown, Kulik, and Conway argued that these special memories involve the limbic system, specifically, the amygdala. There is an abundancy of research that shows the amygdala involvement regarding retrieval of emotional memories, for example, research using brain imaging techniques.
Pharmacological enhancement
In healthy adults, longterm visual episodic memory can be enhanced specifically[20] through administration of the Acetylcholine esterase inhibitor Donepezil, whereas verbal episodic memory can be improved in persons with the val/val genotype of the val158met polymorphism through administration of the CNS penetrant specific catecholamine-O-methyltransferase inhibitor Tolcapone.[21] Furthermore, episodic memory is enhanced through AZD3480, a selective agonist at the neuronal alpha4beta2 nicotinic receptor, which is developed by the company Targacept.[22] Currently, there are several other products developed by several companies—including new catecholamine-O-methyltransferase inhibitors with fewer side effects—that aim for improving episodic memory. A recent placebo controlled study found that DHEA, which is a functional cortisol antagonist, improves episodic memory in healthy young men (Alhaj et al. 2006).[23]
Based on a review of behavioral studies, it is suggested that there may be selective damage to the limbic-prefrontal episodic memory system in some autistic people.[25] Another study exhibited evidence of autistic deficits in the episodic or self-conscious memory of personally experienced events.[26]
The label "amnesia" is most often given to patients with deficits in episodic memory.
A rare type of shellfish poisoning called amnesic shellfish poisoning or "ASP" quite effectively and irreversibly damages the hippocampus, rendering one amnesic.
Tulving (1983) proposed that to meet the criteria of episodic memory, evidence of conscious recollection must be provided. Demonstrating episodic memory in the absence of language, and thus in non-human animals, has been declared impossible as long as there are no agreed-upon non-linguistic behavioral indicators of conscious experience (Griffiths et al., 1999).
This idea was first challenged by Clayton and Dickinson in their work with the western scrub jay (Aphelocoma californica). They were able to demonstrate that these birds may possess an episodic-like memory system as they found that they remember where they cached different food types and discriminately recovered them depending on the perishability of the item and time that elapsed since caching. Thus, scrub-jays appear to remember the "what-where-and-when" of specific past caching events. The authors argued that such performance meets the behavioral criteria for episodic memory, but referred to the ability as "episodic-like" memory because the study did not address the phenomenological aspects of episodic memory.
According to a study conducted by the University of Edinburgh (2006), hummingbirds were the first animal to demonstrate two of the aspects of episodic memory—the ability to recall where certain flowers were located and how recently they were visited. Other studies have examined this type of memory in different animal species, such as dogs,[32][33] rats, honey bees, and primates.
The ability of animals to encode and retrieve past experiences relies on the circuitry of the medial temporal lobe, a structure including the hippocampus.[34] Animal lesion studies have provided significant findings related to the importance of particular brain structures in episodic-like memory. For example, hippocampal lesions have severely impacted all three components (what, where, and when) in animals, suggesting that the hippocampus is responsible for detecting novel events, stimuli, and places when forming new memories and retrieving that information later on.
Despite similar neural areas and evidence from experiments, some scholars remain cautious about comparisons to human episodic memory.[35] Purported episodic-like memory often seems fixed to a particular domain or could be explained in terms of procedural or semantic memory. The problem may be better tractable by studying episodic memory's adaptive counterpart: the capacity to flexibly imagine future events. However, a recent experiment addressed one of Suddendorf and Busby (2003)'s specific criticisms (the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis, which states that nonhuman animals can only take actions based on immediate needs, as opposed to future needs). Correia and colleagues demonstrated [36] that western scrub-jays can selectively cache different types of foods depending on which type of food they will desire at a future time, offering strong evidence against the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis by demonstrating that scrub-jays can flexibly adjust their behavior based on past experience of desiring a particular food. Similarities and differences between humans and other animals are currently much debated.[37]
Autobiographical memory
An autobiographical memory is a personal representation of general or specific events and personal facts. Additionally, it also refers to the memory of a person's history. An individual does not remember exactly everything that has happened in one's past. Memory is constructive, where previous experience affects how we remember events and what we end up recalling from memory. Similarly, autobiographical memory is constructive and reconstructed as an evolving process of history. A person's autobiographical memory is fairly reliable, although the reliability of autobiographical memories is questionable because of memory distortions.[38]
Autobiographical memories can differ for special periods of life. For instance, people recall a few personal events from the first years of their lives. The loss of these first events is called childhood or infantile amnesia. Also, people tend to recall many personal events from adolescence and early adulthood. This effect is called the reminiscence bump. Additionally, people recall many personal events from their previous few years. For adolescents and young adults, the reminiscence bump and the recent events can coincide.[39]
It is known that autobiographical memories initially are stored as episodic memories, but it is currently unknown if autobiographical memories are the same as episodic memories or if the autobiographical memories become converted to semantic memories with time.[40]
Types
Specific events
When you first set foot in the ocean.
General events
What it feels like stepping into the ocean in general. This is a memory of what a personal event is generally like. It might be based on the memories of having stepped in the ocean, many times during the years.
Flashbulb memories
Flashbulb memories are critical autobiographical memories about a major event.
Neural network models
This section needs expansion. You can help by adding to it. (April 2012)
Episodic memories can be stored in autoassociative neural networks (e.g., a Hopfield network) if the stored representation includes information on the spatiotemporal context in which an item was studied. Smaller memories such as words or references said by someone are labeled as inactive or active neurons in the entorhinal cortex.[41][42]
Neural networks help us understand how the brain sends and receives different messages to the body, and how they are connected. These networks are a group of neurons or structures that are connected together. These structures work harmoniously to produce different cognitions within the brain. One of the largest proposals for this ideology is that of Diffusion Tensor Imaging.[43] This technique traces the differing pathways of nerve fibres that further create communication throughout differing structures. These networks can be thought of as neural maps that can expand or contract according to the information being processed at that time. Neural Network Models can undergo learning patterns to use episodic memories to predict certain moments. Neural network models help the episodic memories by capturing the naturalistic state you are currently in such as scenery, rooms, time, smell, or even your current feeling.[44][45]
^Howard MW, Kahana MJ (2002). "When Does Semantic Similarity Help Episodic Retrieval?". Journal of Memory and Language. 46: 85–98. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2798. S2CID65222.
^Scarf D, Gross J, Colombo M, Hayne H (March 2013). "To have and to hold: episodic memory in 3- and 4-year-old children". Developmental Psychobiology. 55 (2): 125–32. doi:10.1002/dev.21004. PMID22213009.
^Grön G, Kirstein M, Thielscher A, Riepe MW, Spitzer M (October 2005). "Cholinergic enhancement of episodic memory in healthy young adults". Psychopharmacology. 182 (1): 170–9. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0043-2. PMID16021483. S2CID21341306.
^Apud JA, Mattay V, Chen J, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Rasetti R, Alce G, Iudicello JE, Akbar N, Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (May 2007). "Tolcapone improves cognition and cortical information processing in normal human subjects". Neuropsychopharmacology. 32 (5): 1011–20. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301227. PMID17063156. S2CID24026336.
^Dunbar G, Boeijinga PH, Demazières A, Cisterni C, Kuchibhatla R, Wesnes K, Luthringer R (May 2007). "Effects of TC-1734 (AZD3480), a selective neuronal nicotinic receptor agonist, on cognitive performance and the EEG of young healthy male volunteers". Psychopharmacology. 191 (4): 919–29. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0675-x. PMID17225162. S2CID10920515.
^Alhaj HA, Massey AE, McAllister-Williams RH (November 2006). "Effects of DHEA administration on episodic memory, cortisol and mood in healthy young men: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study". Psychopharmacology. 188 (4): 541–51. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0136-y. PMID16231168. S2CID25964357.
^Joseph RM, Steele SD, Meyer E, Tager-Flusberg H (2005). "Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory?". Neuropsychologia. 43 (10): 1400–11. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.010. PMID15989932. S2CID10924334.
^Parrott AC, Lees A, Garnham NJ, Jones M, Wesnes K (2016). "Cognitive performance in recreational users of MDMA of 'ecstasy': evidence for memory deficits". Journal of Psychopharmacology. 12 (1): 79–83. doi:10.1177/026988119801200110. PMID9584971. S2CID39651353.
^Jansari, Ashok; Parkin, Alan J. (1996). "Things that go bump in your life: Explaining the reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory". Psychology and Aging. 11 (1): 85–91. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.11.1.85. ISSN0882-7974. PMID8726374.
^Michael E Hasselmo, James L McClelland, Neural models of memory, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, Volume 9, Issue 2, 1999, Pages 184-188, ISSN 0959-4388, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80025-7.
^Khalil, Radwa; Moftah, Marie Z.; Moustafa, Ahmed A. (2017-10-19). "The effects of dynamical synapses on firing rate activity: a spiking neural network model". European Journal of Neuroscience. 46 (9): 2445–2470. doi:10.1111/ejn.13712. ISSN0953-816X. PMID28921686. S2CID41810408.
^Qihong Lu, Uri Hasson, Kenneth A Norman (2022) A neural network model of when to retrieve and encode episodic memories eLife 11:e74445https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.74445
Kart-Teke E, De Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Dere E (March 2006). "Wistar rats show episodic-like memory for unique experiences". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 85 (2): 173–82. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2005.10.002. PMID16290193. S2CID26124449.
Convenzione di Vienna sul diritto dei trattatiPartecipanti alla Convenzione di Vienna Firmato e ratificato Firmato Non firmatoFirma23 maggio 1969 LuogoVienna, Austria Efficacia27 gennaio 1980 CondizioniRatifica di 35 stati Parti116 (ad ottobre 2018) Firmatari45 DepositarioSegretario Generale delle Nazioni Unite Linguearabo, cinese, inglese, francese, russo e spagnolo voci di tratt...
Danny DanonDanon pada Agustus 2012 Perwakilan Permanen Israel untuk Perserikatan Bangsa-Bangsa ke-17PetahanaMulai menjabat 15 Oktober 2015PresidenReuven RivlinPerdana MenteriBenjamin Netanyahu PendahuluRon ProsorPenggantiPetahanaKementerian Sains, Teknologi dan Ruang Angkasa ke-22Masa jabatan14 Mei 2015 – 27 Agustus 2015PresidenReuven RivlinPerdana MenteriBenjamin Netanyahu PendahuluYaakov PeriPenggantiOfir Akunis Informasi pribadiLahir8 Mei 1971 (umur 52)Ramat Gan, Is...
Disambiguazione – Se stai cercando il dispositivo di illuminazione basato sull'arco voltaico, vedi Lampada ad arco. Arcoprodotto di disegno industrialeDati generaliAnno di progettazione1962 ProgettistaPier Giacomo Castiglioni e Achille Castiglioni Profilo prodottoTipo di oggettolampada Ideauna lampada da terra che permetta di avere il punto luce direttamente sopra il tavolo (o la testa) ovviando al sistema di sospensione Concettisemplicità, razionalità, praticità, versatilità Produttor...
مايكل شارمر معلومات شخصية الميلاد 8 سبتمبر 1954 (العمر 69 سنة)غلينديل، كاليفورنيا الإقامة كاليفورنيا، الولايات المتحدة مواطنة الولايات المتحدة الأمريكية الجنسية أمريكي لون الشعر شعر أصهب الحياة العملية المدرسة الأم جامعة ببردين [لغات أخرى] شهادة جامعية دكت�...
The 1st AlbumAlbum studio karya Modern TalkingDirilis1 April 1985Direkam1984GenreSynthpopLabelBMG, HansaProduserDieter BohlenKronologi Modern Talking The 1st Album(1985) Let's Talk About Love(1985)Let's Talk About Love1985 Penilaian profesional Skor ulasan Sumber Nilai Allmusic [1] The 1st Album adalah album musik pertama karya Modern Talking. Album ini dirilis tahun 1985 di Jerman. Daftar lagu You're My Heart, You're My Soul — 5:36 You Can Win If You Want — 3:53 There's Too M...
Pour les articles homonymes, voir Stefan et Étienne. Stefan NemanjaFonctionJoupanBiographieNaissance Vers 1113 ou 1114Depedogen (en) (Dioclée)Décès 13 février 1199Monastère de HilandarSépulture Monastère de StudenicaActivités Moine orthodoxe (1196 - 13 février 1199), chef militaireFamille NemanjićPère ZavidaFratrie TihomirStracimirMiroslav de HumConjoint Anastasie de Serbie (en)Enfants Vukan NemanjićStefan Ier NemanjićSaint SavaAutres informationsÉtape de canonisation Vé...
Questa voce sull'argomento stagioni delle società calcistiche italiane è solo un abbozzo. Contribuisci a migliorarla secondo le convenzioni di Wikipedia. Segui i suggerimenti del progetto di riferimento. Voce principale: Società Sportiva Dilettantistica Baracca Lugo. Ente Sportivo Francesco BaraccaStagione 1936-1937Sport calcio Squadra Baracca Lugo Allenatore Ettore Baruzzi Presidente Carlo Basini Serie C7º posto nel girone D. 1935-1936 1937-1938 Si invita a seguire il modello ...
Government ministry of Bangladesh Ministry of Liberation War AffairsBengali: মুক্তিযুদ্ধ বিষয়ক মন্ত্রণালয় Muktijodho Bishyok MontranaloiGovernment of Bangladesh SealAgency overviewFormed23 October 2001[1]JurisdictionGovernment of BangladeshHeadquartersSecretariat, DhakaMinister responsibleAKM Mozammel Haque[2]Agency executiveMd. Khaza Miah, SecretaryWebsiteMinistry of Liberation War Affairs The Ministry of Liberation Wa...
الدولة الصفارية السلالة الصفارية صفاريون 861 – 1003 الدولة الصفارية في أقصى اتساعها في عهد يعقوب بن الليث الصفار عاصمة زرنج، موجودة حاليًا في أفغانستان نظام الحكم ملكية الديانة أهل السنة والجماعة صفاريون يعقوب بن الليث الصفار 861–879 خلف ابن أحمد 963–1002 التاريخ ال...
Francisco José CróquerBornMay 23, 1920Turmero, Aragua, VenezuelaDiedDecember 18, 1955 (aged 35)Barranquilla, ColombiaOccupation(s)Sportscaster, racing driverYears active1938–1955AwardsVenezuelan Baseball Hall of Fame and Museum induction (2005) Francisco José Cróquer [crocker] (May 23, 1920 – December 18, 1955) was a Venezuelan sportscaster specialized in baseball and boxing. He was popularly known as Pancho Pepe Cróquer.[1][2] Early life Born in Turmero, Aragua,...
For other uses, see Down-low (disambiguation). 1995 single by R. Kelly featuring The Isley BrothersDown Low (Nobody Has to Know)Single by R. Kelly featuring The Isley Brothersfrom the album R. Kelly ReleasedDecember 11, 1995Recorded1995GenreR&BLength4:49LabelJiveSongwriter(s)Robert KellyProducer(s)R. KellyR. Kelly singles chronology You Remind Me of Something (1995) Down Low (Nobody Has to Know) (1995) I Can't Sleep Baby (If I) (1996) The Isley Brothers singles chronology I'm So P...
район / муниципальный районОнежский район Флаг Герб 63°43′ с. ш. 37°27′ в. д.HGЯO Страна Россия Входит в Архангельскую область Включает 8 муниципальных образований Адм. центр город Онега Глава администрации Юрий Александрович Максимов История и география Дата о�...
List of islands of The Bahamas A map of The Bahamas The following is an alphabetical list of the islands and cays of the Commonwealth of The Bahamas.[1][2][3] Contents 0–9 A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z A Abaco Island Abner Cay Abraham's Bay Acklins Island Adderley Cay Alcorine Cay Alder Cay Allan Cays Allans Cay Ambergris Cay(s) Andrew island Andros Island - largest island of the Bahamas Angel Cays Angle and Fish Cay Anna Cay Arawak Cay Araway C...
Traditional Venezuelan rice and bean dish Pabellón criolloPabellón criollo, this is the pabellón con tajadas variation (has fried plantain slices).CourseMealPlace of originVenezuelaRegion or stateLatin AmericaCooking time 40 minutesMain ingredientsrice and beans, shredded beef in stew and stewed Media: Pabellón criollo Black beans with grated cheese, Shredded meat, slices of Plantain and Rice Pabellón criollo (Spanish pronunciation: [paβeˈʝoŋ ˈkɾjo.ʝo]) is a trad...
Dutch painter (1616-1680) Ferdinand BolSelf-portrait (c. 1669) by Ferdinand BolBorn(1616-06-24)24 June 1616Dordrecht, NetherlandsDied24 August 1680(1680-08-24) (aged 64)Herengracht, NetherlandsNationalityDutchKnown forPainting, ethching, draftsmanNotable workPortrait of Elisabeth BasSignature Ferdinand Bol (24 June 1616 - 24 August 1680) was a Dutch painter, etcher and draftsman. Although his surviving work is rare, it displays Rembrandt's influence; like his master, Bol favored his...
Central banking system of the US The Fed redirects here. For the Welsh trade union, see South Wales Miners' Federation. For other uses, see The Fed (disambiguation). Federal Reserve SystemSeal of the Federal Reserve SystemFlag of the Federal Reserve SystemThe Eccles Building in Washington, D.C., which serves as the Federal Reserve System's headquartersHeadquartersEccles Building, Washington, D.C., U.S.EstablishedDecember 23, 1913 (110 years ago) (1913-12-23)Governing bodyBoard o...