Hunting hypothesis

In paleoanthropology, the hunting hypothesis is the hypothesis that human evolution was primarily influenced by the activity of hunting for relatively large and fast animals, and that the activity of hunting distinguished human ancestors from other hominins.

While it is undisputed that early humans were hunters, the importance of this fact for the final steps in the emergence of the genus Homo out of earlier australopithecines, with its bipedalism and production of stone tools (from about 2.5 million years ago), and eventually also control of fire (from about 1.5 million years ago), is emphasized in the "hunting hypothesis", and de-emphasized in scenarios that stress the omnivore status of humans as their recipe for success, and social interaction, including mating behaviour as essential in the emergence of language and culture.

Advocates of the hunting hypothesis tend to believe that tool use and toolmaking essential to effective hunting were an extremely important part of human evolution, and trace the origin of language and religion to a hunting context.

As societal evidence David Buss cites that modern tribal population deploy hunting as their primary way of acquiring food.[1] The Aka pygmies in the Central African Republic spend 56% of their quest for nourishment hunting, 27% gathering, and 17% processing food. Additionally, the !Kung in Botswana retain 40% of their calories from hunting and this percentage varies from 20% to 90% depending on the season.[2] For physical evidence Buss first looks to the guts of humans and apes. The human gut consists mainly of the small intestines, which are responsible for the rapid breakdown of proteins and absorption of nutrients. The ape's gut is primarily colon, which indicates a vegetarian diet. This structural difference supports the hunting hypothesis in being an evolutionary branching point between modern humans and modern primates. Buss also cites human teeth in that fossilized human teeth have a thin enamel coating with very little heavy wear and tear that would result from a plant diet. The absence of thick enamel also indicates that historically humans have maintained a meat-heavy diet.[2] Buss notes that the bones of animals human ancestors killed found at Olduvai Gorge have cut marks at strategic points on the bones that indicate tool usage and provide evidence for ancestral butchers.[2]

Women are theorized to have participated in hunting, either on their own or as a collective group effort.[3] It is suggested that in the past, women targeted low but guaranteed food, whereas men targeted higher risk higher reward food.[4] The Gathering Hypothesis is a view that states men provided the evolution of the current human through hunting while women contributed via gathering.[5] Though criticized by many, it provides clues that both hunting and gathering were patterns of acquiring food and resources.

Applications

Sexual division of labor

According to the hunting hypothesis, women are preoccupied with pregnancy and dependent children and so do not hunt because it is dangerous and less profitable. In addition, subsistence labor differentiates as observations suggests gender patterns originate from genetic traits.[3] Another possible explanation for women gathering is their inherent prioritization of rearing offspring, which is difficult to uphold if women were hunting.[6] Hunting is seen as more cost effective for men than for women.[5] The division of labor allows both types of resources (animals and plants) to be utilized.[5] Individual or small group hunting requires patience and skill more than strength, so women are just as capable as men. Plant collecting can be a physically demanding task so strength, endurance, or patience does not explain why women do not regularly hunt large game.[4] Since women hunt while menstruating, and if a child is still being breastfed, the mother may take him or her along in a shoulder sling while hunting or gathering.[4] Women hunt when it is compatible with children, and this usually means communal net hunts and/or hunting small game, and if childcare prevents a woman from hunting when young, the expertise to be an effective hunter later on may not be acquired.[4]

Women's involvement

Though the hunting hypothesis is still being debated today, many experts have theorized the impact that women had concerning their involvement with hunter-gatherers being primarily males, was much larger than previously thought.[4][7][8][3] Women in foraging societies do hunt small game regularly and, occasionally, large game.[4] The majority of human's evolutionary history consisted of being hunter-gatherers as such women evolved the necessary traits needed for hunting such as endurance, movement coordination, and athleticism.[7] Hunting big game requires a collaborative effort, thus participation from all abled-bodies was encouraged which included females.[3] In addition, Atlatl or Spear-thrower's required more energy to be utilized so contributions from everyone, including females, would've contributed with mitigating the energy exerted to use Atlatl's.[3] Such examples consist of the Martu women in western Australia, for example, who frequently hunt goannas and skink.[4] Women also participate in communal game drives and can have extensive land knowledge as well, which they use to assist their husbands in hunting.[4] Kelly Robert's example consists of 6 Agta women who are hunters and returned home with a kill 31 percent of the time, whereas men averaged 17 percent.[4] The women's expertise with hunting was further shown with mixed groups of male and female hunters being the most successful, coming home with kills 41 percent of the time.[4] Agta females who have reached the end of their childbearing years, those with children old enough to look after themselves in camp, or those who are sterile are the ones who intentionally hunt.[4] It's noted that women target reliable but low-return-rate foods, whereas men target less reliable but high-return-rate foods.[4] This could be an explanation as to why women weren't commonly documented as hunters.

Provisioning hypothesis

Parental investment

Buss purports that the hunting hypothesis explains the high level of human male parental investment in offspring as compared to primates. Meat is an economical and condensed food resource in that it can be brought home to feed the young, as it is not efficient to carry low-calorie food across great distances. Thus, the act of hunting and the required transportation of the kill in order to feed offspring is a reasonable explanation for human male provisioning.[2]

Male coalitions

Buss suggests that the Hunting hypothesis also explains the advent of strong male coalitions. Although chimpanzees form male-male coalitions, they tend to be temporary and opportunistic. Contrastingly, large game hunters require consistent and coordinated cooperation to succeed in large game hunting. Thus male coalitions were the result of working together to succeed in providing meat for the hunters themselves and their families.[2] Kristen Hawkes suggests further that obtaining resources intended for community consumption increases a male's fitness by appealing to the male's society and thus being in the good favor of both males and females. The male relationship would improve hunting success and create alliances for future conflict and the female relationship would improve direct reproductive success.[2] Buss proposes alternate explanations of emergence of the strong male coalitions. He suggests that male coalitions may have been the result of group-on-group aggression, defense, and in-group political alliances. This explanation does not support the relationship between male coalitions and hunting.[2]

Hawkes proposes that hunters pursue large game and divide the kill across the group. Hunters compete to divvy up the kill to signal courage, power, generosity, prosocial intent, and dedication. By engaging in these activities, hunters receive reproductive benefits and respect.[9] These reproductive benefits lead to greater reproductive success in more skilled hunters.[9] Evidence of these hunting goals that do not only benefit the families of the hunters are in the Ache and Hadza men. Hawkes notes that their hunting techniques are less efficient than alternative methods and are energetically costly, but the men place more importance on displaying their bravery, power, and prosocial intent than on hunting efficiency. This method is different as compared to other societies where hunters retain the control of their kills and signal their intent of sharing. This alternate method aligns with the coalition support hypothesis, in efforts to create and preserve political associations.[9]

Reciprocal altruism

The meat from successful large game hunts are more than what a single hunter can consume. Further, hunting success varies by week. One week a hunter may succeed in hunting large game and the next may return with no meat. In this situation Buss suggests that there are low costs to giving away meat that cannot be eaten by the individual hunter on his own and large benefits from the expectation of the returned favor in a week where his hunting is not successful.[2] Hawkes calls this sharing “tolerated theft” and purports that the benefits of reciprocal altruism stem from the result that families will experience “lower daily variation and higher daily average” in their resources.[10]

Provisioning may actually be a form of sexual competition between males for females.[11] Hawkes suggests that male provisioning is a particularly human behavior, which forges the nuclear family.[10] The structure of familial provisioning determines a form of resource distribution. However, Hawkes does acknowledge inconsistencies across societies and contexts such as the fluctuating time courses dedicated to hunting and gathering, which are not directly correlated with return rates, the fact that nutrition value is often chosen over caloric count, and the fact that meat is a more widely spread resource than other resources.[10]

The show-off hypothesis

The show-off hypothesis is the concept that more successful men have better mate options. The idea relates back to the fact that meat, the result of hunting expeditions, is a distinct resource in that it comes in large quantities that more often than not the hunter's own family is not able to consume in a timely manner so that the meat doesn't go sour.[2] Also the success of hunting is unpredictable whereas berries and fruits, unless there is a drought or a bad bush, are fairly consistent in seasonality. Kristen Hawkes argues that women favor neighbors opting for men who provide the advantageous, yet infrequent meat feasts.[10] These women may profit from alliance and the resulting feasts, especially in times of shortage. Hawkes suggests that it would be beneficial for women to reward men who employ the “show-off strategy” by supporting them in a dispute, caring for their offspring, or providing sexual favors.[10] The benefits women may gain from their alignment lie in favored treatment of the offspring spawned by the show-off from neighbors.[10] Buss echoes and cites Hawke's thoughts on the show-off's benefits in sexual access, increased likelihood of having children, and the favorable treatment his children would receive from the other members of the society.[2] Hawkes also suggests that show-offs are more likely to live in large groups and thus be less susceptible to predators.[10] Show-offs gain more benefits from just sharing with their family (classical fitness) in the potential favorable treatment from the community and reciprocal altruism from other members of the community.[10]

Hawkes uses the Ache people of Paraguay as evidence for the Show-off hypothesis. Food acquired by men was more widely distributed across the community and inconsistent resources that came in large quantities when acquired were also more widely shared.[10]

While this is represented in the Ache according to Hawkes, Buss notes that this trend is contradicted in the Hadza who evenly distribute the meat across all members of their population and whose hunters have very little control over the distribution. In the Hadza the show-off hypothesis does not have to do with the resources that result from hunting, but from the prestige and risk that is involved in big game hunting. There are possible circuitous benefits such as protection and defense.[2]

The gathering hypothesis

The Gathering Hypothesis is the view that men provided critical evolutionary propulsion of the modern human through hunting, whereas women contributed via gathering.[5] In addition, it helps provide for the fact that our ancestor's diets consisted mostly of plant food.[5] It's suggested by David Buss that stone tools were invented not strictly for hunting, but for gathering plants and used for digging them up.[5] This could explain the migration from forests to woodlands as tools allowed easy access to previously used methods. As such, this view results in the hunting part of the modern human coming much later.[5] Though women weren't strictly hunters, a woman's time investment in foraging depended on how much food her husband brought back.[5] Gathering plant foods allows a person to return to camp when necessary, but hunting may require an overnight stay so as to continue tracking the animal in the morning.[4]

The gathering hypothesis controversy

The Gathering Hypothesis has been criticized by those who believe it's incapable of explaining our human origins in the primate lineage.[5] A common argument against the Gathering hypothesis is if gathering was the best or most efficient method of acquiring food, then why wouldn’t men just gather and stop wasting their time hunting.[5] The division of labor among men and woman is unaccounted for throughout cultures.[5] Hunting often takes the hunter far away from the home base, selection would favor hunters who could find their way home without getting lost along the way.[5] Locating and gathering edible nuts, berries, fruit, and tubers would require a different set of spatial skills.[5] The high prevalence of male hunters and female gatherers among traditional societies, although not conclusive evidence, provides one more clue that both activities are part of the human pattern of procuring food.[5]

See also

References

  1. ^ Buss, David (1999). Evolutionary Psychology: The new Science of the Mind. New York: Needham Heights: Allyn & Bacon.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Buss, David M. Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 2011. Print. 80
  3. ^ a b c d e Haas, Randall; Watson, James; Buonasera, Tammy; Southon, John; Chen, Jennifer C.; Noe, Sarah; Smith, Kevin; Llave, Carlos Viviano; Eerkens, Jelmer; Parker, Glendon (2020-11-06). "Female hunters of the early Americas". Science Advances. 6 (45): eabd0310. Bibcode:2020SciA....6..310H. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd0310. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 7673694. PMID 33148651.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m Kelly, Robert L. (2013). The lifeways of hunter-gatherers : the foraging spectrum. Robert L. Kelly (2nd ed.). Cambridge. ISBN 978-1-107-34172-2. OCLC 836848791.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  5. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n M., Buss, David (2019). Evolutionary psychology : the new science of the mind. Routledge. ISBN 978-1-138-08818-4. OCLC 1295429010.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ Stoet, Gisbert (2011). "Sex Differences in Search and Gathering Skills". Evolution and Human Behavior. 32 (6): 416–22. Bibcode:2011EHumB..32..416S. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.03.001.
  7. ^ a b M., Buss, David (2019). Evolutionary psychology : the new science of the mind. Routledge. ISBN 978-1-138-08818-4. OCLC 1295429010.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Meehan, Marjorie C. (1976-09-06). "The Hunting Hypothesis: A Personal Conclusion Concerning the Evolutionary Nature of Man". JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 236 (10): 1172. doi:10.1001/jama.1976.03270110068039. ISSN 0098-7484.
  9. ^ a b c Nolin, David A (2010). "Food-Sharing Networks in Lamalera, Indonesia". Human Nature. 21 (3): 243–68. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.003. PMC 3398706. PMID 22822299.
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i Hawkes, Kristen (1991). "Showing Off Tests of an Hypothesis About Men's Foraging Goals". Ethology and Sociobiology. 12 (1): 29–54. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(91)90011-E.
  11. ^ Larsen, Clark S. (2012). "Chapter 10: Early Hominid Origins and Evolution: The Roots of Humanity". Our Origins: Discovering Physical Anthropology. W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0-393-93498-4. OCLC 813933300.

Read other articles:

Dr. Ir.Salis Subhi AprilianMSc. [[President Director & CEO Badak LNG]] 13Masa jabatan2015 – 2017 PendahuluRachmad HardadiPenggantiDidik Sasongko WidiPresiden Direktur Pertamina EPMasa jabatan2009 – 2011Presiden Direktur PT Pertamina EP CepuMasa jabatan2008 – 2009 Informasi pribadiLahir21 April 1963 (umur 60) Purwokerto, Jawa Tengah, IndonesiaAlma materInstitut Teknologi BandungTexas A&M University Tanda tanganSunting kotak info • L ...

 

 

Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Legiun Mangkunegaran – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTORLegiun Mangkunegaran prajurit resmi Kadipaten Mangkunegaran. Legiun Mangkunegaran adalah korps angkatan bersenjata Kadipaten Mangku...

 

 

Kuda laut ekor harimau Hippocampus comes Status konservasiRentanIUCN41008 TaksonomiKerajaanAnimaliaFilumChordataKelasActinopteriOrdoSyngnathiformesFamiliSyngnathidaeGenusHippocampusSpesiesHippocampus comes Cantor, 1850 lbs Kuda laut ekor harimau (Hippocampus comes) adalah sebuah spesies ikan dalam keluarga Syngnathidae. Spesies tersebut pertama kali dideskripsikan oleh Theodore Cantor pada tahun 1850.[2] Spesies tersebut ditemukan di India, Indonesia, Malaysia, Filipina, Singapura, Th...

Yutu-2Robot Yutu-2 di atas permukaan Bulan dilihat dari Chang'e 4Jenis misiRobot bulanOperatorBadan Antariksa Nasional TiongkokDurasi misi3 bulan (direncanakan)[1] Kini: 1912 hari Properti wahanaMassa luncurRobot: 140 kg[2]Massa mendarat140 kgDimensi1.5 × 1.0 × 1.0 m[3] Awal misiTanggal luncur7 Desember 2018, 18:23 UTC[4]Roket peluncurLong March 3B[5][6]Tempat peluncuranPusat Peluncuran Satelit Xichang Robot penjelajah BulanTanggal pendar...

 

 

River in Spain For the river in Bolivia, see Guadalquivir River (Bolivia). GuadalquivirMontoro situated on a bend of the river.Location of the GuadalquivirEtymologyfrom الوادي الكبير (al-wādī l-kabīr), the great valley or the great river in ArabicLocationCountrySpainRegionAndalusiaCitiesCórdoba, SevillePhysical characteristicsSourceCañada de las Fuentes • locationCazorla Range, Quesada, Jaén MouthAtlantic Ocean • locationAlmonte (Huelva) ...

 

 

† Человек прямоходящий Научная классификация Домен:ЭукариотыЦарство:ЖивотныеПодцарство:ЭуметазоиБез ранга:Двусторонне-симметричныеБез ранга:ВторичноротыеТип:ХордовыеПодтип:ПозвоночныеИнфратип:ЧелюстноротыеНадкласс:ЧетвероногиеКлада:АмниотыКлада:Синапсиды�...

Chambéry Koordinat: 45°34′12″N 5°54′42″E / 45.57°N 5.9118°E / 45.57; 5.9118NegaraPrancisArondisemenChambéryKantonKota utama dari 4 kantonAntarkomuneGrand ChambéryKode INSEE/pos73065 /  Chambéry ialah sebuah komune di Prancis yang berpenduduk 0.900 jiwa (2004) yang termasuk wilayah departemen Savoie, di region Rhône-Alpes. Kota ini telah menjadi ibu kota historis kawasan Savoia sejak abad ke-13, saat Amedeo V dari Savoia menjadikannya sebagai pusat...

 

 

Website that reports on website outages DowndetectorOwnerOokla[1]URLdowndetector.comLaunchedApril 2012; 12 years ago (2012-04)[2]Current statusOnline Downdetector is an online platform that provides users with real-time information about the status of various websites and services. The information that is provided by the site is based upon user outage reports, which are collected from various sources, including the page for each website on Downdetec...

 

 

Pandawa Water WorldLogo Pandawa Water WorldLokasiSukoharjo, Jawa Tengah, IndonesiaDibuka22 Desember 2007Luas area12,3 ekar (5,0 ha)Situs webwww.pandawa-lima.com Pandawa Water World (bahasa Jawa: ꦠꦶꦂꦠꦧꦸꦮꦤꦦꦤ꧀ꦝꦮ 'Tirta Buwana Pandhawa') merupakan sebuah taman wisata air yang berada di Solo Baru - Sukoharjo, Jawa Tengah. Tempat wisata ini bertemakan tokoh pewayangan Mahabharata dan diresmikan pada 22 Desember 2007.[1] Lokasi Pandawa Water World terletak...

2004 terrorist attack in Indonesia Australian embassy bombing in JakartaPart of terrorism in IndonesiaFloral tributes in front of the Australian Embassy in Jakarta a few days after the bombingLocation6°13′5.4″S 106°49′52″E / 6.218167°S 106.83111°E / -6.218167; 106.83111 (Jakarta Embassy Bombing)Jakarta, IndonesiaDate9 September 2004; 19 years ago (2004-09-09) 10:30 am (UTC +7)TargetAustralian embassyAttack typeCar bombingDeath...

 

 

Pengepungan Konstantinopel (1204)Bagian dari Perang Salib KeempatTentara Salib Memasuki Konstantinopel, oleh Eugène Delacroix.Tanggal8–13 April 1204LokasiKonstantinopel, Kekaisaran Romawi TimurHasil Kemenangan besar Tentara SalibPerubahanwilayah Konstantinopel direbut Tentara SalibPihak terlibat Kekaisaran Romawi Timur Tentara SalibTokoh dan pemimpin Alexios V Doukas Bonifacio I Enrico DandoloKekuatan Romawi Timur: 15.000 pasukan,[1] Romawi Timur: 20 kapal[2] Tentara Salib:...

 

 

此條目翻譯品質不佳。翻譯者可能不熟悉中文或原文語言,也可能使用了機器翻譯。請協助翻譯本條目或重新編寫,并注意避免翻译腔的问题。明顯拙劣的翻譯請改掛{{d|G13}}提交刪除。  「希拉克」重定向至此。關於法国洛泽尔省的同名市镇,請見「希拉克 (洛泽尔省)」。 雅克·勒内·希拉克Jacques René Chirac 第22任法國總統安道爾大公任期1995年5月17日—2007年5月16日...

Cet article concerne l'activité commerciale. Pour les autres significations, voir Commerce (homonymie). « Commercial » redirige ici. Pour l'activité professionnelle lié à la vente, voir Commercial (métier). Si ce bandeau n'est plus pertinent, retirez-le. Cliquez ici pour en savoir plus. Cet article ne cite pas suffisamment ses sources (décembre 2016). Si vous disposez d'ouvrages ou d'articles de référence ou si vous connaissez des sites web de qualité traitant du th�...

 

 

Cyberattack surrounding PlayStation Network 2011 PlayStation Network outagePlayStation Network logoDateApril 20 – May 14, 2011 (2011-04-20 – 2011-05-14)Duration24 days (3 weeks and 3 days)TypeExternal intrusion, data breachTargetPlayStation Network and Qriocity servicesOutcome Services restored after 23 days of downtime, with compensation offered to PSN users Personal data exposed for 77 million PlayStation Network accounts $171 million in costs for Sony The ...

 

 

Political party National States' Rights Party Flag of the National States' Rights Party, based on the Confederate battle flagLeaderEdward Reed FieldsJ. B. StonerFounderEdward Reed FieldsFounded1958Dissolved1987HeadquartersKnoxville, TennesseeMembership (1970)150IdeologyNeo-fascismSegregationismWhite supremacyAntisemitismPolitical positionFar-rightColorsRed, blue, and white(party and flag colors) The National States' Rights Party was a white supremacist[1] political party that bri...

1970–February 1974 Parliament of the United Kingdom ←1966–1970 Parliament February–October 1974 Parliament→Palace of Westminster in 1973OverviewLegislative bodyParliament of the United KingdomTerm18 June 1970 (1970-06-18) – 12 March 1974 (1974-03-12)Election1970 United Kingdom general electionGovernmentHeath ministry House of CommonsMembers630SpeakerHorace KingSelwyn Lloyd LeaderWilliam WhitelawRobert CarrJim Prior Prime M...

 

 

Questa voce è orfana, ovvero priva di collegamenti in entrata da altre voci. Inseriscine almeno uno pertinente e utile e rimuovi l'avviso. Segui i suggerimenti del progetto di riferimento. Questa voce sull'argomento scrittori venezuelani è solo un abbozzo. Contribuisci a migliorarla secondo le convenzioni di Wikipedia. Luz Machado Luz Machado, nota anche con lo pseudonimo di Ágata Cruz (Ciudad Bolívar, 3 febbraio 1916 – Caracas, 11 agosto 1999), è stata una scrittrice, giornalist...

 

 

Cette page d’homonymie répertorie les différents sujets et articles partageant un même nom. Sur les autres projets Wikimedia : Sixte, sur le Wiktionnaire Pour l’article ayant un titre homophone, voir Sixt. Venant du latin sextus qui signifie sixième[1], Sixte peut désigner : Anthroponyme Sixte est un prénom masculin ; son équivalent féminin est Sixtine. Saint Sixte est un hagionyme porté par plusieurs saints du christianisme. Les personnes réelles et les personna...

Wilhelm Reich (1897-1957), Ia adalah seorang Psikiater dari Wina yang mengembangkan sistem psikoanalisis yang berkonsentrasi pada keseluruhan struktur karakter, bukan pada gejala neurotik individu saja Wilhelm Reich (lahir, 24 Maret 1897 di Austria-Hungaria [sekarang di Ukraina] - meninggal, 3 November 1957, Lewisburg, Amerika Serikat) adalah seorang psikiater dari Wina yang mengembangkan sistem psikoanalisis yang berkonsentrasi pada keseluruhan struktur karakter, bukan pada gejala neurotik i...

 

 

NucetKotaLetak NucetNegara RumaniaProvinsiProvinsi BihorStatusKotaPemerintahan • Wali kotaCostel Nelu Sirghie (area_total_km2=41.11)Populasi (2002) • Total2.399Zona waktuUTC+2 (EET) • Musim panas (DST)UTC+3 (EEST)Situs webhttp://www.primarianucet.groupromo.ro/ Nucet (pelafalan dalam bahasa Rumania: /nu.'ʧet/, bahasa Hungaria: Diófás) adalah kota yang terletak di Provinsi Bihor, Transilvania barat, Rumania. Nama Nucet berarti pohon walnut da...