Framing effect (psychology)

The framing effect is a cognitive bias in which people decide between options based on whether the options are presented with positive or negative connotations.[1] Individuals have a tendency to make risk-avoidant choices when options are positively framed, while selecting more loss-avoidant options when presented with a negative frame. In studies of the bias, options are presented in terms of the probability of either losses or gains. While differently expressed, the options described are in effect identical. Gain and loss are defined in the scenario as descriptions of outcomes, for example, lives lost or saved, patients treated or not treated, monetary gains or losses.[2]

Prospect theory posits that a loss is more significant than the equivalent gain,[2] that a sure gain (certainty effect and pseudocertainty effect) is favored over a probabilistic gain,[3] and that a probabilistic loss is preferred to a definite loss.[2] One of the dangers of framing effects is that people are often provided with options within the context of only one of the two frames.[4]

The concept helps to develop an understanding of frame analysis within social movements, and also in the formation of political opinion where spin plays a large role in political opinion polls that are framed to encourage a response beneficial to the organization that has commissioned the poll. It has been suggested that the use of the technique is discrediting political polls themselves.[5] The effect is reduced, or even eliminated, if ample credible information is provided to people.[5]

Research

Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman explored how different phrasing affected participants' responses to a choice in a hypothetical life and death situation in 1981.[2]

Participants were asked to choose between two treatments for 600 people affected by a deadly disease. Treatment A was predicted to result in 400 deaths, whereas treatment B had a 33% chance that no one would die but a 66% chance that everyone would die. This choice was then presented to participants either with positive framing, i.e. how many people would live, or with negative framing, i.e. how many people would die.

Framing Treatment A Treatment B
Positive "Saves 200 lives" "A 33% chance of saving all 600 people, 66% possibility of saving no one."
Negative "400 people will die" "A 33% chance that no people will die, 66% probability that all 600 will die."

Treatment A was chosen by 72% of participants when it was presented with positive framing ("saves 200 lives") dropping to 22% when the same choice was presented with negative framing ("400 people will die").

A recent study was done on the framing effect in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Researchers found that the framing effect was larger in this context than under normal circumstances, indicating that individuals were more influenced by how options were presented during the pandemic. There was a positive association between the framing effect and perceived stress and concerns related to coronavirus, indicating that these factors are influential when it comes to decision-making. However, they were not related to risk aversion.[6]

This effect has been shown in other contexts:

  • 93% of PhD students registered early when a penalty fee for late registration was emphasized, but only 67% did so when this was presented as a discount for earlier registration.[7]
  • 62% of people disagreed with allowing "public condemnation of democracy", but only 46% of people agreed that it was right to "forbid public condemnation of democracy".[8]
  • More people will support an economic policy if the employment rate is emphasised than when the associated unemployment rates is highlighted.[5]
  • It has been argued that pretrial detention may increase a defendant's willingness to accept a plea bargain, since imprisonment, rather than freedom, will be their baseline, and pleading guilty will be viewed as an event that will cause their earlier release rather than as an event that will put them in prison.[9]

Extensionality violation

In logic, extensionality requires "two formulas which have the same truth-value under any truth-assignments to be mutually substitutable salva veritate in a sentence that contains one of these formulas."[10] Put simply, objects that have the same external properties are equal. This principle, applied to decision making, suggests that making a decision in a problem should not be affected by how the problem is described. For example, varied descriptions of the same decision problem should not give rise to different decisions, due to the extensionality principle. If judgments are made on the basis of irrelevant information as described, that is called an extensionality violation. Addressing extensionality violations entails cultivating awareness of how different descriptions of a problem may inadvertently influence decisions, and as a result developing strategies to mitigate such deviations. In doing so, decision-makers can aim to uphold the extensionality principle as a guide in navigating the complexity of choice, focusing on decisions that are more in tune to the inherent properties of the problem rather than its descriptions.[10]

Developmental factors

The framing effect has consistently been shown to be one of the largest biases in decision making.[11] In general, susceptibility to framing effects increases with age. Age difference factors are particularly important when considering health care[12][13][14] and financial decisions. The susceptibility to framing can influence how older individuals perceive and in turn respond to information, potentially leading to less optimal choices that can have lasting consequences. In healthcare, for instance, where decisions profoundly affect well-being, the framing effect can sway older individuals towards or away from certain treatment options based on the way in which the medical information is presented. Likewise, in financial decision-making, the framing of retirement planning or investment risks may have significant impacts on the choices individuals make, potentially impacting their financial security and state in the later stages of their lives.[15]

However, the framing effect seems to disappear when encountering it in a foreign (non-native) language.[16]: 246 [17] One explanation of this disappearance is that a non-native language provides greater cognitive and emotional distance than one's native tongue.[18] A foreign language is also processed less automatically than a native tongue. This leads to more deliberation, which can affect decision making, resulting in decisions that are more systematic.[19]

Childhood and adolescence

Framing effects in decision-making become stronger as children age.[20][21][22] This is partially because qualitative reasoning increases with age.[20] While preschoolers are more likely to make decisions based on quantitative properties, such as probability of an outcome, elementary schoolers and adolescents become progressively more likely to reason qualitatively, opting for a sure option in a gain frame and a risky option in a loss frame regardless of probabilities.[20] The increase in qualitative thinking is related to an increase in "gist based" thinking that occurs over a lifetime.[23]

However, qualitative reasoning, and thus susceptibility to framing effects, is still not as strong in adolescents as in adults,[20][22] and adolescents are more likely than adults to choose the risky option under both the gain and loss frames of a given scenario.[21] One explanation for adolescent tendencies toward risky choices is that they lack real-world experience with negative consequences, and thus over-rely on conscious evaluation of risks and benefits,[21] focusing on specific information and details or quantitative analysis.[24] This reduces influence of framing effects and leads to greater consistency across frames of a given scenario.[24] Children between the ages of 10 and 12 are more likely to take risks and show framing effects, while younger children only considered the quantitative differences between the two options presented.[25]

Young adulthood

Younger adults are more likely than older adults to be enticed by risk-taking when presented with loss frame trials. This is a noteworthy phenomenon that underscores the complex interplay between age and decision-making tendencies.[11]

In multiple studies of undergraduate students, researchers have found that students are more likely to prefer options framed positively. This could be attributed to a variety of factors such as an inclination for novelty-seeking, a more optimistic outlook on outcomes, or even a reduced aversion to risk which is inherent in youth.[26] For example, they are more likely to enjoy meat labeled 75% lean meat as opposed to 25% fat, or use condoms advertised as being 95% effective as opposed to having a 5% risk of failure.[26]

Young adults are especially susceptible to framing effects when presented with an ill-defined problem in which there is no correct answer and individuals must arbitrarily determine what information they consider relevant.[26] For example, undergraduate students are more willing to purchase an item such as a movie ticket after losing an amount equivalent to the item's cost than after losing the item itself.This susceptibility underscores the importance of considering psychological factors in the context of decision-making. Recognizing this vulnerability emphasizes the need for decision-makers to be aware of cognitive biases when navigating decision-making in which there isn't a clear answer so that they can take a more informed approach.[26]

Older adulthood

The framing effect is claimed to be greater in older adults than in younger adults or adolescents.[12][13][27] This claim may be a result of enhanced negativity bias,[13] though some sources claim that the negativity bias actually decreases with age.[15]

In particular, this increased susceptibility to the framing effect manifested itself mainly in response to negative frames. Positive framings were not found to have a significant effect on the framing effect in older adults.[27] This may be due in part to socioemotional selectivity theory, where the increased age shifts the focus of adults from risk taking to maximizing their emotional experiences in the present, hence the increased framing in the negative frame.[27] The dual process theory may also play a role as negative framings evoke less heightened responses, leading to the deployment of the implicit processes. The implicit process is found to be frame-sensitive, and thus may be the reason why framing is pronounced in negative frames for older adults.[27][28]

Another possible cause is that older adults have fewer cognitive resources available to them and are more likely to default to less cognitively demanding strategies when faced with a decision.[11] They tend to rely on easily accessible information, or frames, regardless of whether that information is relevant to making the decision in question.[11] Several studies have shown that younger adults will make less biased decisions than older adults because they base their choices on interpretations of patterns of events and can better employ decision making strategies that require cognitive resources like working-memory skills. Older adults, on the other hand, make choices based on immediate reactions to gains and losses.[11]

Older adults' lack of cognitive resources, such as flexibility in decision making strategies, may cause older adults to be influenced by emotional frames more so than younger adults or adolescents.[29] In addition, as individuals age, they make decisions more quickly than their younger counterparts.[11] It is significant that, when prompted to do so, older adults will often make a less biased decision with reevaluation of their original choice.[11][12]

The increase in framing effects among older adults has important implications, especially in medical contexts.[12][13][14][27] Older adults are influenced heavily by the inclusion or exclusion of extraneous details, meaning they are likely to make serious medical decisions based on how doctors frame the two options rather than the qualitative differences between the options, causing older adults to inappropriately form their choices.[11]

When considering cancer treatments, framing can shift older adults' focus from short- to long-term survival under a negative and positive frame, respectively.[12] When presented with treatment descriptions described in positive, negative, or neutral terms, older adults are significantly more likely to agree to a treatment when it is positively described than they are to agree to the same treatment when it is described neutrally or negatively.[13] Additionally, framing often leads to inconsistency in choice: a change in description qualities after an initial choice is made can cause older adults to revoke their initial decision in favor of an alternative option.[13] Older adults also remember positively framed statements more accurately than negatively framed statements.[12][30] This has been demonstrated by evaluating older adults' recall of statements in pamphlets about health care issues.[12][30]

See also

References

  1. ^ Plous 1993
  2. ^ a b c d Tversky & Kahneman 1981
  3. ^ Clark 2009
  4. ^ Druckman 2001a
  5. ^ a b c Druckman 2001b
  6. ^ Rachev, Nikolay R.; Han, Hyemin; Lacko, David; Gelpí, Rebekah; Yamada, Yuki; Lieberoth, Andreas (2021-09-10). "Replicating the Disease framing problem during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic: A study of stress, worry, trust, and choice under risk". PLOS ONE. 16 (9): e0257151. Bibcode:2021PLoSO..1657151R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0257151. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 8432807. PMID 34506543.
  7. ^ Gächter et al. 2009
  8. ^ Rugg, as cited in Plous 1993
  9. ^ Bibas, Stephanos (2004). "Plea Bargaining outside the Shadow of Trial". Harvard Law Review. 117 (8): 2463–47. doi:10.2307/4093404. JSTOR 4093404. SSRN 464880.
  10. ^ a b Bourgeois-Gironde, Sacha; Giraud, Raphaël (2009). "Framing effects as violations of extensionality" (PDF). Theory and Decision. 67 (4): 385–404. doi:10.1007/s11238-009-9133-7. ISSN 0040-5833. S2CID 17963795.
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h Thomas, A. K.; Millar, P. R. (2011). "Reducing the Framing Effect in Older and Younger Adults by Encouraging Analytic Processing". The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 67B (2): 139–49. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr076. hdl:10.1093/geronb/gbr076. PMID 21964668.
  12. ^ a b c d e f g Erber, Joan (2013). Aging and Older Adulthood (3 ed.). John Wiley & Sons. p. 218. ISBN 978-0-470-67341-6. Archived from the original on 2017-08-10. Retrieved 2014-11-03.
  13. ^ a b c d e f Peters, Ellen; Finucane, Melissa; MacGregor, Donald; Slovic, Paul (2000). "The Bearable Lightness of Aging: Judgment and Decision Processes in Older Adults" (PDF). In Paul C. Stern; Laura L. Carstensen (eds.). The aging mind: opportunities in cognitive research. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press. ISBN 978-0-309-06940-3.
  14. ^ a b Hanoch, Yaniv; Thomas Rice (2006). "Can Limiting Choice Increase Social Welfare? The Elderly and Health Insurance". The Milbank Quarterly. 84 (1): 37–73. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0009.2006.00438.x. JSTOR 25098107. PMC 2690155. PMID 16529568.
  15. ^ a b Carpenter, S. M.; Yoon, C. (2011). "Aging and consumer decision making". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1235 (1): E1–12. Bibcode:2011NYASA1235E...1C. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06390.x. PMC 3799963. PMID 22360794.
  16. ^ Zuckermann, Ghil'ad (2020). Revivalistics: From the Genesis of Israeli to Language Reclamation in Australia and Beyond. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780199812790. ISBN 9780199812776
  17. ^ Keysar, B.; Hayakawa, S. L.; An, S. G. (2012). "The Foreign-Language Effect: Thinking in a Foreign Tongue Reduces Decision Biases". Psychological Science. 23 (6): 661–68. doi:10.1177/0956797611432178. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 22517192. S2CID 1397617.
  18. ^ Keysar, Boaz; Hayakawa, Sayuri; An, Sun Gyu (2012). "The Foreign-Language Effect : Thinking in a Foreign Tongue Reduces Decision Biases" (PDF). Psychological Science. 23 (6): 661–8. doi:10.1177/0956797611432178. PMID 22517192. S2CID 1397617. Archived from the original (PDF) on 23 November 2015. Retrieved 3 December 2014.
  19. ^ Keysar, Boaz; Hayakawa, Sayuri; An, Sun Gyu (2012). "The Foreign-Language Effect : Thinking in a Foreign Tongue Reduces Decision Biases" (PDF). Psychological Science. 23 (6): 661–8. doi:10.1177/0956797611432178. PMID 22517192. S2CID 1397617. Archived from the original (PDF) on 23 November 2015. Retrieved 3 December 2014.
  20. ^ a b c d Reyna, V. F.; Farley, F. (2006). "Risk and Rationality in Adolescent Decision Making: Implications for Theory, Practice, and Public Policy". Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 7 (1): 1–44. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00026.x. PMID 26158695. S2CID 16591695.
  21. ^ a b c Albert, D.; Steinberg, L. (2011). "Judgment and Decision Making in Adolescence". Journal of Research on Adolescence. 21: 211. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00724.x.
  22. ^ a b Strough, J.; Karns, T. E.; Schlosnagle, L. (2011). "Decision-making heuristics and biases across the life span". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1235 (1): 57–74. Bibcode:2011NYASA1235...57S. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06208.x. PMC 3755606. PMID 22023568.
  23. ^ Reyna, V. F. (2008). "A Theory of Medical Decision Making and Health: Fuzzy Trace Theory". Medical Decision Making. 28 (6): 850–65. doi:10.1177/0272989X08327066. PMC 2617718. PMID 19015287.
  24. ^ a b Schlottmann, A.; Tring, J. (2005). "How Children Reason about Gains and Losses: Framing Effects in Judgement and Choice". Swiss Journal of Psychology. 64 (3): 153. doi:10.1024/1421-0185.64.3.153.
  25. ^ Boyer, T. (2006). "The development of risk-taking: A multi-perspective review". Developmental Review. 26 (3): 291–345. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2006.05.002.
  26. ^ a b c d Revlin, Russell (2012). "Chapter 11: Solving Problems". Cognition: Theory and Practice. New York, New York: Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-0716756675.
  27. ^ a b c d e Xu, Lance (2024-02-06). "Advanced age predicts increased susceptibility to attribute, goal, and risky-choice framing in negative frame valences". Humanities and Social Sciences Communications. 11 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1057/s41599-024-02658-6. ISSN 2662-9992.
  28. ^ Roberts, Ian D.; Teoh, Yi Yang; Hutcherson, Cendri A. (January 2022). "Time to Pay Attention? Information Search Explains Amplified Framing Effects Under Time Pressure". Psychological Science. 33 (1): 90–104. doi:10.1177/09567976211026983. ISSN 0956-7976. PMC 8985223. PMID 34860637.
  29. ^ Watanabe, S.; Shibutani, H. (2010). "Aging and decision making: Differences in susceptibility to the risky-choice framing effect between older and younger adults in Japan". Japanese Psychological Research. 52 (3): 163. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5884.2010.00432.x.
  30. ^ a b Löckenhoff, C. E. (2011). "Age, time, and decision making: From processing speed to global time horizons". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1235 (1): 44–56. Bibcode:2011NYASA1235...44L. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06209.x. PMC 3757097. PMID 22023567.

Sources

Further reading

  • Kühberger, Anton; Tanner, Carmen (2010). "Risky choice framing: Task versions and a comparison of prospect theory and fuzzy-trace theory". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 23 (3): 314–29. doi:10.1002/bdm.656.

Read other articles:

Article principal : Déchets du BTP. Déchets de démolition Les déchets de démolition sont des déchets provenant de la démolition de bâtiments, de routes, de ponts ou d'autres structures[1]. Les débris varient en composition, mais les principaux composants, en poids (aux États-Unis) comprennent le béton, les produits en bois, les bardeaux d'asphalte, les blocs et les briques, l'acier et les cloisons sèches[2]. Il existe un potentiel de recyclage de nombreux éléments des déch...

 

 

Arkamelvi Karmani Pa Sahli Tk. III Kasad Bidang Wassus dan LHPetahanaMulai menjabat 9 November 2023 PendahuluHendrikus Joko RiantoPenggantiPetahanaKepala Staf Komando Garnisun Tetap I/JakartaMasa jabatan29 Maret 2023 – 9 November 2023 PendahuluRano TilaarPenggantiEdi SaputraDirektur Latihan Kodiklat TNIMasa jabatan25 September 2020 – 29 Maret 2023 PendahuluTri MartonoPenggantiKhairul Anwar Mandailing Informasi pribadiLahir10 Februari 1967 (umur 57)Jakarta, Indon...

 

 

MünsterPanorama Münster BenderaLambang kebesaranLetak Münster NegaraJermanNegara bagianNordrhein-WestfalenWilayahMünsterKreisDistrik perkotaanDidirikan793Subdivisions6Pemerintahan • Lord MayorMarkus Lewe (CDU) • Governing partiesCDULuas • Total302,28 km2 (11,671 sq mi)Ketinggian60 m (200 ft)Populasi (2019-12-31)[1] • Total315.293 • Kepadatan10/km2 (27/sq mi)Zona waktuWET/WMPET (UTC+1/+2)...

Kl 35 Klemm Kl 35D Role Two-seat sports and training aircraftType of aircraft Manufacturer Klemm Leightflugzeugbau Gmbh Designer Friedrich Fecher First flight 1935 Introduction 1935 Status out of service Primary users LuftwaffeCzechoslovakian Air Force Royal Hungarian Air Force Royal Romanian Air Force Produced 1937-1944 Number built c.2,000? The Klemm Kl 35 is a German sporting and training aeroplane developed as a successor to the Kl 25. A product of Klemm Leichtflugzeugbau Gmbh it sh...

 

 

Cet article concerne un événement en cours. Ces informations peuvent manquer de recul, changer à mesure que l’événement progresse ou ne pas prendre en compte des développements récents. Le titre lui-même peut être provisoire. N’hésitez pas à l’améliorer en veillant à citer vos sources.La dernière modification de cette page a été faite le 5 mars 2024 à 14:56. Coalition internationale en Irak et en Syrie Pendant la seconde guerre civile irakienne et la guerre civile syri...

 

 

This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Peanuts animated specials – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2019) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Peanuts animated specialsCharles Schulz's Hollywood walk of fame star. It features the 'television receiver' honor, whi...

Vins de glace canadiens rouges (assez rarement produits) Le vin de glace (connu dans les régions productrices sous les noms de Eiswein et icewine) est un vin fait à partir de raisins vendangés gelés. Ce vin possède naturellement une forte teneur en sucres résiduels, équilibrée par son acidité. Histoire C'est vers la fin du XVIIIe siècle, en Autriche et en Allemagne que surpris par des gelées précoces, des vignerons furent obligés de presser des raisins glacés et découvrire...

 

 

American singer-songwriter (1935-2004) Max D. BarnesBarnes and Merle Haggard performing in Nashville, Tennessee.Background informationBirth nameMax Duane BarnesBorn(1935-07-24)July 24, 1935Hard Scratch, Iowa, U.S.DiedJanuary 11, 2004(2004-01-11) (aged 68)Nashville, Tennessee, U.S.GenresCountryYears active1960–2004LabelsOvation, Polydor, Country Roads Records (UK)Musical artist Max Duane Barnes (July 24, 1935 – January 11, 2004) was an American country singer and songwriter born i...

 

 

Державний комітет телебачення і радіомовлення України (Держкомтелерадіо) Приміщення комітетуЗагальна інформаціяКраїна  УкраїнаДата створення 2003Керівне відомство Кабінет Міністрів УкраїниРічний бюджет 1 964 898 500 ₴[1]Голова Олег НаливайкоПідвідомчі ор...

土库曼斯坦总统土库曼斯坦国徽土库曼斯坦总统旗現任谢尔达尔·别尔德穆哈梅多夫自2022年3月19日官邸阿什哈巴德总统府(Oguzkhan Presidential Palace)機關所在地阿什哈巴德任命者直接选举任期7年,可连选连任首任萨帕尔穆拉特·尼亚佐夫设立1991年10月27日 土库曼斯坦土库曼斯坦政府与政治 国家政府 土库曼斯坦宪法 国旗 国徽 国歌 立法機關(英语:National Council of Turkmenistan) ...

 

 

American politician William Haile26th Governor of New HampshireIn officeJune 4, 1857 – June 2, 1859Preceded byRalph MetcalfSucceeded byIchabod GoodwinMember of the New Hampshire House of RepresentativesIn office1846–185018531856Member of the New Hampshire SenateIn office1854–1856 Personal detailsBornMay 1807 (1807-05)Putney, Vermont, U.S.DiedJuly 22, 1876 (1876-07-23) (aged 69)Keene, New Hampshire, U.S.Political partyRepublicanSpouseSabrina Shaw WalkerProfess...

 

 

سباق الزمن في بطولة العالم لسباق الدراجات على الطريق تفاصيل السباقالرياضةسباق الدراجات على الطريقبداية1994عدد المواسم29 (في 2022)الفئةبطولة العالم لسباق الدراجات على الطريقالتصنيفCM‏ الجوائزالأحدث توبياس فوسالأكثر فوزًا طوني مارتين فابيان كانسيليرا(4 انتصارات)السباقات ا...

Dominican baseball player (born 1991) For the politician, see José Rafael Molina Ureña. In this Spanish name, the first or paternal surname is Ureña and the second or maternal family name is Rodríguez. Baseball player José UreñaUreña with the Miami Marlins in 2019Texas Rangers – No. 54PitcherBorn: (1991-09-12) September 12, 1991 (age 32)Santo Domingo, Dominican RepublicBats: RightThrows: RightMLB debutApril 14, 2015, for the Miami MarlinsMLB statistics (th...

 

 

Anjing gembala Anatolia Seekor anjing gembala Anatolia di Prancis Nama lain Anjing Anatolia Anjing kepala hitam Anjing kepala hitam Anatolia Anjing Kangal Negara asal Turki Ciri-ciri Berat Jantan 50–65 kg (110–143 pon) Betina 40–55 kg (88–121 pon) Tinggi Jantan 74–81 cm (29–32 in) Betina 71–79 cm (28–31 in) Bulu Pendek, kasar Jumlah anak 5–10 ekor anak anjing Masa hidup 13–15 tahun Klasifikasi & standar FCI Grup 2 Seksi 2 #331 s...

 

 

Disambiguazione – Se stai cercando altri significati, vedi Four Corners (disambigua). Il punto esatto di intersezione dei quattro stati La regione Four Corners messa in evidenza Stare in quattro stati differenti è un'azione comune per i turisti Four Corners (dall'inglese: Quattro angoli) è l'unico punto del territorio degli Stati Uniti d'America in cui i confini di quattro stati si toccano: Arizona, Colorado, Nuovo Messico e Utah. Indice 1 Descrizione 2 Monumento 3 Altri casi 4 Curiosit�...

Spanish civil order Order of Isabella the Catholic Breast star of the OrderAwarded by the Spanish MonarchTypeOrder of merit, KnighthoodEstablished1815; 209 years ago (1815)MottoA LA LEALTAD ACRISOLADA (To Proven Loyalty) and POR ISABEL LA CATÓLICA (For Isabella the Catholic)Awarded forActions in benefit to Spain and the CrownStatusCurrently constitutedGrand MasterKing Felipe VIGrand ChancellorJosé Manuel Albares, Minister of Foreign AffairsGrades Knight/Dame of the Collar ...

 

 

Theatre and opera company in Dortmund, Germany Theater DortmundTheater 1904, destroyed in World War II, and now (Opera House)Formation1904 (1904) as Stadttheater DortmundTypemunicipal theatrical organizationLocationDortmundCoordinates51°30′40″N 7°27′42″E / 51.51111°N 7.46167°E / 51.51111; 7.46167Websitewww.theaterdo.de Theater Dortmund is a theatrical organization that produces operas, musicals, ballets, plays, and concerts in Dortmund, Germany. It was...

 

 

このフィクションに関する記事は、ほとんどがあらすじ・登場人物のエピソードといった物語内容の紹介だけで成り立っています。 製作過程や社会的影響、専門家による批評や分析など、作品外部の情報の加筆を行い、現実世界の観点を説明してください。(2024年5月) (使い方) ONE PIECE > ONE PIECEの用語一覧 ONE PIECEの用語一覧(ワンピースのようごいちらん)では...

إذاعة الجيش الإسرائيلي الشعار المكتوب (بالعبرية: מה שקורה עכשיו)‏  تاريخ أول بث 1950[1]  إحداثيات جهاز البث 32°03′00″N 34°45′22″E / 32.050027777778°N 34.756055555556°E / 32.050027777778; 34.756055555556   الموقع الإلكتروني الموقع الرسمي  تعديل مصدري - تعديل   أحد اقسام الاذاعة غاليه...

 

 

1946 film The Mysterious Mr. MDirected byLewis D. CollinsVernon KeaysWritten byPaul HustonJoseph F. PolandStarringRichard MartinPamela BlakeDennis MooreEdmund MacDonaldByron FoulgerCinematographyGus PetersonDistributed byUniversal PicturesRelease date August 1, 1946 (1946-08-01)[1] Running time13 chapters (227 minutes)CountryUnited StatesLanguageEnglish The Mysterious Mr. M is a 1946 Universal Pictures movie serial, the 137th and last serial produced by Universal. Plot ...