Egocentric bias is the tendency to rely too heavily on one's own perspective and/or have a higher opinion of oneself than reality.[1] It appears to be the result of the psychological need to satisfy one's ego and to be advantageous for memory consolidation. Research[by whom?] has shown[citation needed][weasel words] that experiences, ideas, and beliefs are more easily recalled when they match one's own, causing an egocentric outlook. Michael Ross and Fiore Sicoly first identified this cognitive bias in their 1979 paper, "Egocentric Biases in Availability and Attribution".[2][3] Egocentric bias is referred to by most psychologists as a general umbrella term under which other related phenomena fall.
The effects of egocentric bias can differ based on personal characteristics, such as age and the number of languages one speaks. Thus far, there have been many studies focusing on specific implications of egocentric bias in different contexts. Research on collaborative group tasks have emphasized that people view their own contributions differently than they view that of others. Other areas of research have been aimed at studying how mental health patients display egocentric bias, and at the relationship between egocentric bias and voter distribution. These types of studies surrounding egocentric bias usually involve written or verbal questionnaires, based on the subject's personal life or their decision in various hypothetical scenarios.
History and analysis
The term "egocentric bias" was first coined in 1980 by Anthony Greenwald, a psychologist at The Ohio State University.[4] He described it as a phenomenon in which people skew their beliefs so that what they recall from their memory or what they initially understood is different than what actually occurred. He cites research by Rogers, Kuiper, and Kirker, who explain that the self-reference effect is the ability of people to recall information better if they think about how the information will affect them during the encoding process (recording memories in their brain). Greenwald argues that the self-reference effect causes people to exaggerate their role in a situation. Furthermore, information is better encoded, and thus people are more likely to suffer from egocentric bias, if they produce information actively rather than passively, such as by having a direct role in the outcome of a situation.
Egocentric bias occurs when people fail to consider situations from other people's perspectives. Egocentric bias has influenced ethical judgements to the point where people not only believe that self-interested outcomes are preferential but are also the morally sound way to proceed.[5] People are more inclined to be aware of their own behaviors since they can use their thoughts and emotions to gain more information about themselves. These thoughts and emotions can affect how people view themselves in relation to others in specific situations. A common example arises when people are asked to explain how much credit should be given to each person in a collaborative project. Daniel Schacter, a psychology professor at Harvard University, considers egocentric bias as one of the "seven sins" of memory and essentially reflects the prominent role played by the self when encoding and retrieving episodic memories. As such, people often feel that their contributions to a collaborative project are greater than those of other members, since people tend to focus more on how much they have done.[6]
In social context, egocentric bias influences people to choose a social circle that is capable of maintaining one's positive traits. Studies show that one's choice of friend or social circle is likely to be dependent on the amount of positive feedback received.[7]
Examples
In a 1993 study conducted in Japan, subjects were asked to write down fair or unfair behaviors that they themselves or others did. When writing about fair behavior, they tended to start with the word "I" rather than "others". Likewise, they began unfair behaviors with "others" rather than "I". This demonstrates that people tend to attribute successes and positive behaviors to themselves, while placing the burden of failures and negative behaviors on others.[6] Furthermore, in this study there were gender differences detected; Japanese women, compared to men, remembered the behaviors of others more than their own, and were also more probable to characterize fair or unfair behavior to others compared to themselves.[6]
Another study found that egocentric bias influences perceived fairness. Subjects felt that overpayment to themselves were more fair than overpayment to others; by contrast, they felt the underpayment to themselves were less fair than underpayment to others. Greenberg's studies showed that this egocentrism was eliminated when the subjects were put in a self-aware state, which was applied in his study with a mirror being placed in front of the subjects. When a person is not self-aware, they perceive that something can be fair to them but not necessarily fair to others. Therefore, fairness was something biased and subjective. When a person is self-aware, there is a uniform standard of fairness and there is no bias. When made self-aware, subjects rated overpayment and underpayment to both themselves and to others as equally unfair. It is believed that these results were obtained because self-awareness elevated subjects' concerns about perceived fairness in payment, thereby overriding egocentric tendencies.[8]
The egocentric bias can also be clearly observed in young children, especially those who have not yet developed theory of mind, or the ability to understand concrete situations from the perspective of others. In one study by Wimmer and Perner, a child and a stuffed animal were presented with two differently colored boxes and both are shown that one contains an object of interest. The experimenter then removed the stuffed animal from the room and moved the object into the other box. When asked where the stuffed animal should search for the object, the children overwhelmingly tended to point to the box that they knew the object was in. Rather than thinking about the animal's perspective, the children displayed an egocentric bias in assuming that the animal would share their point of view, even though the animal had no way of knowing the same information as them.[9]
Causes
The causes and motivations for egocentric bias were investigated in a 1983 journal entry by Brian Mullen of Murray State University. Inspired by the study by Ross et al. demonstrating the false consensus effect, Mullen's paper focused on the overestimation of consensus. Mullen analyzed the NBC television show "Play the Percentages" to determine whether egocentric bias was rooted in a perceptual and unintentional distortion of reality versus a conscious, intentional motivation to appear normalized. Subjects in this analysis were contestants from the show, 20–30 year old middle class married couple with equal gender distribution. At the start of each show, studio audiences were asked several trivia questions, and the percentage of correct answers was recorded for later use in the game. During each round of the game, opposing contestants estimated the percentage of correct answers. The contestant who had a closer estimate wins the percentage of correct answer as a score, and then if they answer said trivia question correctly, wins the remaining percentage for a maximum possible 100 points. The first couple to win 300 points received a cash prize, with the opportunity to win more prizes in bonus rounds. Thus, the show provided incentive for unbiased estimates of consensus. Statistical analysis of the collected data showed that the "egocentric bias of false consensus was observed in spite of the potent incentive for unbiased estimates of consensus." This analysis ultimately supports the hypothesis that egocentric bias is a result of unintentional perceptual distortion of reality rather than a conscious, intentional motivation to appear normalized.[10]
From a psychological standpoint, memories appear to be stored in the brain in an egocentric manner: the role of oneself is magnified in one's experiences to make them more personally relevant and thereby easier to recall. Early childhood memories, therefore, may be more difficult to recall since one's sense of self is less developed, so old memories do not connect as strongly to oneself as newer ones.[4] Moreover, egocentric bias may have evolved from hunter-gatherer times, in which communities were small and interdependent enough that individuals could assume that others around them had very similar outlooks. An egocentric view would have reduced cognitive load and increased communication efficiency.[11]
Effects of personal characteristics
Age
A 2016 study published by Riva, Triscoli, Lamm, Carnaghi, and Silani found that egocentric bias tends to be experienced in a much greater degree by adolescents and older adults than by young and middle aged adults. They examined the emotional effect of visuo-tactile stimulation on pairs of participants from a population of 114 female of varying ages. The varying degree of egocentric bias with age was attributed to the developmental cycle of the right supramarginal gyrus (rSMG) of the parietal lobe, which finishes developing at the end of adolescence and decays early.[12]
Bilingualism
Recent studies of egocentric bias have been done in many different subgroups of people, such as bilingual people. A study done by Paula Rubio-Fernández and Sam Glucksberg found that bilingual people are less prone to egocentric bias because they have grown to pay more attention to others' thoughts. Thus, it is less difficult for them to differentiate between their own opinions and those of others.[13]
Considered to be a facet of egocentric bias, the false-consensus effect states that people believe their thoughts, actions, and opinions are much more common than they are in reality.[10] When people are asked to make an estimate of a population's statistic, they often only have data from themselves and tend to assume that others in the population are similar to them due to egocentric bias. In turn, people tend to overestimate the extent to which their opinion is shared by the rest of the population. Moreover, people tend to believe that those who differ in opinion must be part of a minority and that the majority actually agrees with them. Therefore, the false-consensus effect, or the tendency to deduce judgements from one's own opinions, is a direct result of egocentric bias.[14]
A well known example of false-consensus effect is a study published by Ross, Greene and House in 1977.[15] Students are asked to walk around a campus with a sandwich board that bearing the word "repent". People who agreed to do so (50%) estimated that most of their peers would also agree to do so (average estimation 63.5%). Conversely, those who refused to do the experiment reported that most of their peers would refuse as well.[16]
People who exhibit the false consensus effect take egocentric bias a step further: they not only forgo thinking of other perspectives, but they believe that their viewpoints are those accepted by the majority of people. Nevertheless, some psychologists do not distinguish between egocentric bias and the false consensus effect. For example, in the paper published by Ross, Greene, and House, the terms "false consensus" and "egocentric attribution bias" are used interchangeably.[15] In the second part of their study, they gave out a questionnaire which asked participants which option (out of two choices) they would choose in specified situations, and what percentage of the population would choose which option. In all four scenarios that were given, subjects rated the option that they chose as the most probable. Ross, Greene, and House conclude that their results support the false consensus hypothesis, and that "intuitive estimates of deviance and normalcy, and the host of social inferences and interpersonal responses that accompany such estimates, are systematically and egocentrically biased in accord with his own behavioral choices."[15]
A related concept to egocentric bias is self-serving bias, in which one takes undue credit for achievements and blames failures on external forces. However, egocentric bias differs from self-serving bias in that egocentric bias is rooted in an erroneous assumption of other's perception of reality, while self-serving bias is an erroneous perception of one's own reality. For example, consider a student who earns a low grade in a class. Self-serving bias would result in the assumption that the student's low grade is a result of poor teaching, which would direct the fault of one's reality away from one's own actions.
Egocentric bias might also result in an overestimation of the number of students that received low grades in the class for the purpose to normalize these students' performance. However, similar to the false-consensus effect, the self-serving bias and the egocentric bias have also been used as interchangeable terms.[17]
Both concepts may be the product of individualistic cultures that usually stress independence and personal achievement over group-oriented success. Cross-cultural studies have found a strong presence of the egocentric bias in the primarily individualistic American, South African, and Yugoslavian communities, but noted the opposite effect in the collectivistic Japanese, Nepali, and Indian societies. People from these cultures tend to demonstrate a bias toward modesty, in which success is attributed to external or group-related factors and failures are seen as the result of personal shortcomings.[17]
Bayesian inference
Bayesian reasoning is a form of statistical inference that relies on Bayes' rule to make probability prediction based on given information.[18] In Bayesian updating, people use prior probabilities to make estimates, and then gradually change these probabilities as they gain more information. Bayesian inference is often used by psychologists to determine whether subjects who exhibit the false-consensus effect have a rational thought process. To understand Bayes' rule, consider an example from an experiment by Kreuger and Clement: there is an urn with 100 chips, some blue and some red, and then subjects are told that the first chip drawn from the urn is blue.[19] Subjects are asked to estimate the probability that the urn contains predominantly blue chips. Using Bayes' rule, the probability that a blue chip is drawn given that the urn contains predominantly blue chips is equal to the probability of the urn being predominantly blue multiplied by the probability of the urn being predominantly blue given that a blue chip was drawn, all divided by the probability that the urn is predominantly blue. Most participants overestimated the requested probability. Data shows that subjects tend not to pay attention to sample size when making probability predictions. For example, although it has statistically been proven by the law of large numbers that larger samples have less variability, people tend to claim that large and small samples have the same amount of variability. Studies like the urn experiment above provide evidence that the false-consensus effect is not entirely rational, and that egocentric viewpoints tend to be predominant.
Real-world implications
Collaboration
Egocentric bias can lead to the devaluation of peer contributions and the amplification of one's own work when in a collaborative setting. For example, when group members have been asked to report what percentage of the output they created, the total summed to greater than 100%. Usually, individuals are more easily able to recall their personal contributions and thus believe them to greater or more important. This applies to both positive and negative inputs: in a study of married couples, each spouse rated themselves as more responsible for helpful (cleaning) and detractive activities (causing arguments).[20] Research has shown that feelings of sibling caregivers and their siblings depend on the contact between siblings and their feelings of closeness.[21] Each of these two groups believed that their siblings contributed less to the needs of their family than themselves, and were more resistant to increasing these types of contributions. The closer that siblings were to each other, measured through observation and self reports, the smaller the extent of egocentric bias they felt in reporting each sibling's contribution.
Mental health
An overly exaggerated or extremely low demonstration of egocentric bias could be an indicator of mental illness. Those with anxiety tend to view themselves as the center of all events around them, regardless of their nature or how unrelated they are to oneself. On the other hand, people suffering from depression may have a lower tendency towards egocentricity, as evidenced by the fact that they tend to more realistically rate their contributions to group work, while non-depressed participants often overreport their additions.[4]
Voting
The egocentric bias has also been shown to contribute to a citizen's decision to vote in elections. Firstly, people tend to view their personal choice between voting and abstinence as a reflection of those who support the same candidates and issues. Secondly, although each individual vote has very little power in large-scale elections, those who vote overestimate the significance of their ballot.[22] Moreover, citizens demonstrate egocentric bias, in conjunction with the false-consensus effect, in their predictions of election outcomes. A study examining the 2008 American presidential election found that the more strongly people favor a certain candidate, the higher they estimate that candidate's likelihood of winning the election. For instance, those who strongly preferred Barack Obama predicted that he had a 65% chance of becoming the president, while those who preferred another candidate approximated that he only had a 40% chance of victory.[23]
Citations
^Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Wegner, Daniel M. (2011). Psychology (2nd ed.). Macmillan. p. 254. ISBN978-1-4292-3719-2.
^ abcTanaka, Ken'ichiro (1993). "Egocentric bias in perceived fairness: Is it observed in Japan?". Social Justice Research. 6 (3): 273–285. doi:10.1007/BF01054462. S2CID145436920.
^Greenberg, Jerald (1983). "Overcoming Egocentric Bias in Perceived Fairness Through Self-Awareness". Social Psychology Quarterly. 46 (2): 152–156. doi:10.2307/3033852. JSTOR3033852.
^Wimmer, Heinz; Perner, Josef (1983-01-01). "Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception". Cognition. 13 (1): 103–128. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. PMID6681741. S2CID17014009.
^ abMullen, Brian (1983). "Egocentric Bias in Estimates of Consensus". The Journal of Social Psychology. 121 (1): 31–38. doi:10.1080/00224545.1983.9924463.
^Rubio-Fernández, Paula; Glucksberg, Sam (2012-01-01). "Reasoning about other people's beliefs: Bilinguals have an advantage". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 38 (1): 211–217. doi:10.1037/a0025162. PMID21875251. S2CID31894308.
^Engelmann, Dirk; Strobel, Martin (2001). "The False Consensus Effect Disappears if Representative Information and Monetary Incentives Are Given". Experimental Economics. 3 (3): 241–260. CiteSeerX10.1.1.535.9974. doi:10.1007/bf01669774. S2CID40137442.
^Krueger, Joachim; Russell W., Clement (October 1994). "The truly false consensus effect: An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (4): 596–610. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.596. PMID7965607.
^Lerner, Melvin J.; Tierney, Mary; Somers, Darryl G.; Reid, David; Chiriboga, David (1991). "Adult Children as Caregivers: Egocentric Biases in Judgments of Sibling Contributions". The Gerontologist. 31 (6) (31 ed.): 746–755. doi:10.1093/geront/31.6.746. PMID1800247.
^Acevedo, Melissa; Krueger, Joachim I. (2004-02-01). "Two Egocentric Sources of the Decision to Vote: The Voter's Illusion and the Belief in Personal Relevance". Political Psychology. 25 (1): 115–134. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00359.x. S2CID32654299.
Savitsky, Ken (2007). "Egocentric bias". In Baumeister, Roy F.; Vohs, Kathleen D. (eds.). Encyclopedia of Social Psychology. SAGE Publications. ISBN978-1-4522-6568-1.
Thompson, Suzanne C.; Kelley, Harold H. (1981). "Judgments of responsibility for activities in close relationships". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 41 (3): 469–477. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.3.469.
Air Canada Penerbangan 143Air Canada Penerbangan 143 mendarat di Gimli.Ringkasan kecelakaanTanggal23 July 1983RingkasanKehabisan bahan bakar di udara, Kelalaian PerawatanLokasidi atas Quebec, Ontario dan Manitoba, KanadaPenumpang60Awak8Cedera10Tewas0Selamat69 (all)Jenis pesawatBoeing 767-200OperatorAir CanadaRegistrasiC-GAUNAsalMontreal-Dorval International AirportPerhentianBandara Ottawa Macdonald-Cartier InternasionalTujuanBandara Internasional Edmonton Gimli Glider adalah sebuah nama ...
حرب الاستقلال التركية جزء من مرحلة ما بعد الحرب العالمية الأولى في اتجاه عقارب الساعة من أعلى اليسار:- اجتماع الوفد في مؤتمر سيواس لتحديد أهداف الحركة الوطنية التركية؛ مدنيون أتراك يحملون ذخائر في الجبهة؛ قواي ملّيه [الإنجليزية]؛ سلاح الفرسان التركي يلاحق العدو؛ استعادة �...
Ithaca, New YorkNegaraAmerika SerikatNegara bagianNew YorkCountyTompkins CountyFounded1790Incorporated1888Pemerintahan • MayorSvante Myrick (D)Populasi (2000) • Kota29.287 (city proper) • Metropolitan100.018Zona waktuUTC-5 (EST) • Musim panas (DST)UTC-4 (EDT)Situs webhttp://www.ci.ithaca.ny.us Kota Ithaca (berasal dari nama sebuah pulau Ithaca di Yunani) terletak di tepi pantai bagian selatan Danau Cayuga di tengah negara bagian New York da...
Artikel ini membutuhkan rujukan tambahan agar kualitasnya dapat dipastikan. Mohon bantu kami mengembangkan artikel ini dengan cara menambahkan rujukan ke sumber tepercaya. Pernyataan tak bersumber bisa saja dipertentangkan dan dihapus.Cari sumber: Negara kesatuan – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR (Desember 2023) Gaya penulisan artikel atau bagian ini mengandung kata bersayap yang mengandung opini.Silakan lihat halaman pembicaraan. Lih...
Buildings and structures in St. Augustine, Florida The King's Bakery, 1936 or 1937 The King's Bakery, December 2012 The King's Bakery is a coquina stone structure in St. Augustine, Florida, built during the British colonial period in the state (1763–1783). The building, located on Marine Street, with the rear facing Matanzas Bay, was constructed to supply bread to the British troops quartered across the street at the St. Francis Barracks, a building which formerly housed Franciscan friars, ...
Airport in Salt Lake County, Utah South Valley Regional Airport2006 USGS airphotoIATA: noneICAO: noneFAA LID: U42SummaryAirport typePublicOperatorSalt Lake City Department of AirportsLocationWest Jordan, UtahElevation AMSL4,607 ft / 1,404.2 mCoordinates40°37′10″N 111°59′34″W / 40.61944°N 111.99278°W / 40.61944; -111.99278[1]Websitehttps://slcairport.com/about-the-airport/general-aviation/south-valley-regional-airport/MapU42Location of...
IndrapuriKecamatanNegara IndonesiaProvinsiAcehKabupatenAceh BesarPemerintahan • CamatIrda Junaidi, S.E., M.MPopulasi • Total23,153 /2.018 jiwaKode Kemendagri11.06.03 Kode BPS1108030 Luas197,04 Km2 (19.704 Ha)Desa/kelurahan52 Gambar masjid Indrapuri pada tahun 1896 Stasiun kereta api Indrapuri (tahun 1892-1922) Indrapuri adalah sebuah kecamatan di Kabupaten Aceh Besar, Provinsi Aceh, Indonesia. Posisi geografisnya sangat strategis untuk industri, pertanian, perkebu...
Mai KurakiInformasi latar belakangNama lainMai-KLahir28 Oktober 1982 (umur 41)Asal Funabashi Prefektur ChibaGenreJ-pop, R&BPekerjaanPenyanyiTahun aktif1999 - sekarangLabelBip! Records(1999)Giza Studio(1999-2007)Northern Music(2007-)Artis terkaitŌno Aika (pencipta lagu)Situs webwww.mai-kuraki.com Mai Kuraki (倉木 麻衣code: ja is deprecated , Kuraki Mai, lahir 28 Oktober 1982) adalah penyanyi Jepang beraliran J-pop dan R&B. Singel debutnya, Love, Day After Tomorrow...
Material fibers about 5–10 μm in diameter composed of carbon This article is about loose or woven carbon filament. For the rigid composite material made from carbon fiber used in aerospace and other applications, see Carbon fiber reinforced polymer. This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Carbon fibers – news · new...
Mikhail BakuninLahir(1814-05-30)30 Mei 1814Pryamukhino, Tver Governorate (masa-kini Distrik Kuvshinovsky), Kekaisaran RusiaMeninggal1 Juli 1876(1876-07-01) (umur 62)Bern, SwissEraFilsafat abad ke-19KawasanFilsafat RusiaFilsafat BaratAliranAnarkisme (Kolektivis anarkisme) Dipengaruhi Hegel, Proudhon, Marx Memengaruhi Kropotkin, Malatesta, Chomsky Mikhail Alexandrovich Bakunin (bahasa Rusia: Михаил Александрович Бакунин; 30 Mei 1814 – 1 Juli 1...
Classical ballet company Royal Danish BalletThe Royal Danish Theatre in Copenhagen, principal venue of the Royal Danish BalletGeneral informationNameRoyal Danish BalletLocal nameDen Kongelige BalletYear founded1748FounderKing Frederick VPrincipal venueRoyal Danish TheatreWebsitekglteater.dkArtistic staffBallet MasterNikolaj HübbeOtherOrchestraRoyal Danish OrchestraOfficial schoolRoyal Danish Ballet school The Royal Danish Ballet (Danish: Den Kongelige Ballet) is an internationally renowned c...
War enthusiasm of some Germans at the start of WW1 An enthusiastic crowd cheers soldiers after their mobilisation in Lübeck in 1914. The Spirit of 1914 (German: Geist von 1914; or, more frequently, Augusterlebnis, lit. 'August Experience') was the name given to the feeling of euphoria that affected parts of the German population at the start of World War I. For many decades after the war, the enthusiasm was portrayed as nearly universal, but studies since the 1970s have shown that it w...
Designation given to a Hindu temple priest who performs puja A pujari performing the puja rituals in Varanasi, India. Pūjari is a designation given to a Hindu temple priest who performs pūja. The word comes from the Sanskrit word पूजा meaning worship. They are responsible for performing temple rituals, including pūjā and aarti. Pujari are mainly drawn from the Hindu Brahmin and Billava[1][2][3] History This section is empty. You can help by adding to it. (N...
Kepangeranan WalesTywysogaeth Cymru1216–1542 Bendera Wales Lambang Llewellyn Peta Kepangeranan Wales (1267–1277) yang menggambarkan wilayah yang dikuasai langsung oleh Pangeran Wales. Gwynedd, kepangeranan Llywelyn ap Gruffudd Wilayah yang ditaklukan Llywelyn ap Gruffudd Wilayah vassal Llywelyn Ketuanan baron Marcher Ketuanan Raja InggrisStatusNegara klien Kerajaan Inggris (1277–1542)Ibu kotaAbergwyngregyn dan DolwyddelanBahasa...
Hundreds of Empire ships were employed by the Government of the United Kingdom.[1] They were acquired from a number of sources: many were built for the government; others obtained from the United States; still others were captured or seized from enemy powers. Empire ships were mostly used during World War II by the Ministry of War Transport (MoWT), which owned the ships but contracted out their management to various shipping lines; however, some ships requisitioned during the Suez Cr...
2023 UEFA Women's Under-17 Championship2023. aasta U-17 naiste Euroopa meistrivõistlused jalgpallisTournament detailsHost countryEstoniaDates14–26 MayTeams8 (from 1 confederation)Venue(s)4 (in 3 host cities)Final positionsChampions France (1st title)Runners-up SpainTournament statisticsMatches played15Goals scored65 (4.33 per match)Attendance10,836 (722 per match)Top scorer(s) Liana Joseph Maeline Mendy Vicky López(5 goals each)Best player(s) Vicky Lópe...
Fly91 IATA ICAO Kode panggil IC[1] GOA[2] FLY91 Didirikan9 Januari 2023; 20 bulan lalu (2023-01-09)[3][4]Mulai beroperasi18 Maret 2024; 6 bulan lalu (2024-03-18)[5]Pusat operasiBandar Udara Internasional Manohar (Goa)Armada2[6]Tujuan7[7]Perusahaan indukJust Udo Aviation Pvt. Ltd.[8]Kantor pusatAlcon House, Kadamba Plateau Road, Ribandar, South Goa district, Goa, India[9]Tokoh utamaHarsha Raghavan, Chairman, foun...
Abstract calculator This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages) This article is written like a manual or guide. Please help rewrite this article and remove advice or instruction. (August 2020) This article is written like a research paper or scientific journal. Please help improve the article by rewriting it in encyclopedic style and simplify overly technical phrases. (August 2020) (Learn how ...
فارندورف علم شعار الإحداثيات 51°57′14″N 7°59′36″E / 51.953888888889°N 7.9933333333333°E / 51.953888888889; 7.9933333333333 [1] تقسيم إداري البلد ألمانيا[2][3] خصائص جغرافية المساحة 176.88 كيلومتر مربع (31 ديسمبر 2017)[4] ارتفاع 63 متر عدد السكان عدد الس...