Numeracy

Children in Laos have fun as they improve numeracy with "Number Bingo". They roll three dice, construct an equation from the numbers to produce a new number, then cover that number on the board, trying to get four in a row.
Number bingo improves math skills. LPB Laos.

Numeracy is the ability to understand, reason with, and apply simple numerical concepts.[1] The charity National Numeracy states: "Numeracy means understanding how mathematics is used in the real world and being able to apply it to make the best possible decisions...It's as much about thinking and reasoning as about 'doing sums'". Basic numeracy skills consist of comprehending fundamental arithmetical operations like addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. For example, if one can understand simple mathematical equations such as 2 + 2 = 4, then one would be considered to possess at least basic numeric knowledge. Substantial aspects of numeracy also include number sense, operation sense, computation, measurement, geometry, probability and statistics.[citation needed] A numerically literate person can manage and respond to the mathematical demands of life.[2][3]

By contrast, innumeracy (the lack of numeracy) can have a negative impact. Numeracy has an influence on healthy behaviors, financial literacy, and career decisions. Therefore, innumeracy may negatively affect economic choices, financial outcomes, health outcomes, and life satisfaction.[3][4][5] It also may distort risk perception in health decisions.[6] Greater numeracy has been associated with reduced susceptibility to framing effects, less influence of nonnumerical information such as mood states, and greater sensitivity to different levels of numerical risk.[7] Ellen Peters and her colleagues argue that achieving the benefits of numeric literacy, however, may depend on one's numeric self-efficacy or confidence in one's skills.[8]

Representation of numbers

Humans have evolved to mentally represent numbers in two major ways from observation (not formal math).[9] These representations are often thought to be innate[10] (see Numerical cognition), to be shared across human cultures,[11] to be common to multiple species,[12] and not to be the result of individual learning or cultural transmission. They are:

  1. Approximate representation of numerical magnitude, and
  2. Precise representation of the quantity of individual items.

Approximate representations of numerical magnitude imply that one can relatively estimate and comprehend an amount if the number is large (see Approximate number system). For example, one experiment showed children and adults arrays of many dots.[11] After briefly observing them, both groups could accurately estimate the approximate number of dots. However, distinguishing differences between large numbers of dots proved to be more challenging.[11]

Precise representations of distinct items demonstrate that people are more accurate in estimating amounts and distinguishing differences when the numbers are relatively small (see Subitizing).[11] For example, in one experiment, an experimenter presented an infant with two piles of crackers, one with two crackers the other with three. The experimenter then covered each pile with a cup. When allowed to choose a cup, the infant always chose the cup with more crackers because the infant could distinguish the difference.[11]

Both systems—approximate representation of magnitude and precise representation quantity of individual items—have limited power. For example, neither allows representations of fractions or negative numbers. More complex representations require education. However, achievement in school mathematics correlates with an individual's unlearned approximate number sense.[13]

Definitions and assessment

Fundamental (or rudimentary) numeracy skills include understanding of the real number line, time, measurement, and estimation.[6] Fundamental skills include basic skills (the ability to identify and understand numbers) and computational skills (the ability to perform simple arithmetical operations and compare numerical magnitudes).

More sophisticated numeracy skills include understanding of ratio concepts (notably fractions, proportions, percentages, and probabilities), and knowing when and how to perform multistep operations.[6] Two categories of skills are included at the higher levels: the analytical skills (the ability to understand numerical information, such as required to interpret graphs and charts) and the statistical skills (the ability to apply higher probabilistic and statistical computation, such as conditional probabilities).

A variety of tests have been developed for assessing numeracy and health numeracy.[6][7][14][15][16][17][18] Different tests have been developed to evaluate health numeracy. Two of these tests that have been found to be "reliable and valid" are the GHNT-21 and GHNT-6.[19]

Childhood influences

The first couple of years of childhood are considered to be a vital part of life for the development of numeracy and literacy.[20] There are many components that play key roles in the development of numeracy at a young age, such as Socioeconomic Status (SES), parenting, Home Learning Environment (HLE), and age.[20]

Socioeconomic status

Children who are brought up in families with high SES tend to be more engaged in developmentally enhancing activities.[20] These children are more likely to develop the necessary abilities to learn and to become more motivated to learn.[20] More specifically, a mother's education level is considered to have an effect on the child's ability to achieve in numeracy. That is, mothers with a high level of education will tend to have children who succeed more in numeracy.[20]

A number of studies have, moreover, proved that the education level of the mother is strongly correlated with the average age of getting married. More precisely, females who entered the marriage later, tend to have greater autonomy, chances for skills premium and level of education (i.e. numeracy). Hence, they were more likely to share this experience with children.[21]

Parenting

Parents are advised to collaborate with their child in simple learning exercises, such as reading a book, painting, drawing, and playing with numbers. On a more expressive note[clarification needed], the act of using complex language, being more responsive towards the child, and establishing warm interactions are recommended to parents with the confirmation of positive numeracy outcomes.[20] When discussing beneficial parenting behaviors, a feedback loop is formed because pleased parents are more willing to interact with their child, which in essence promotes better development in the child.[20]

Home-learning environment

Along with parenting and SES, a strong home-learning environment increases the likelihood of the child being prepared for comprehending complex mathematical schooling.[22] For example, if a child is influenced by many learning activities in the household, such as puzzles, coloring books, mazes, or books with picture riddles, then they will be more prepared to face school activities.[22]

Age

Age is accounted for when discussing the development of numeracy in children.[22] Children under the age of 5 have the best opportunity to absorb basic numeracy skills.[22] After the age of seven, achievement of basic numeracy skills become less influential.[22] For example, a study was conducted to compare the reading and mathematical abilities between children of ages five and seven, each in three different mental capacity groups (underachieving, average, and overachieving). The differences in the amount of knowledge retained were greater between the three different groups aged five than between the groups aged seven. This reveals that those of younger ages have an opportunity to retain more information, like numeracy. According to Gelman and Gallistel in The Child's Understanding of Number, 'children as young as 2 years can accurately judge numerosity provided that the numerosity is not larger than two or three'. Children as young as three have been found to understand elementary mathematical concepts.[23] Kilpatrick and his colleagues state 'most preschoolers show that they can understand and perform simple addition and subtraction by at least 3 years of age'.[24] Lastly, it has been observed that pre-school children benefit from their basic understanding of 'counting, reading and writing of numbers, understanding of simple addition and subtraction, numerical reasoning, classifying of objects and shapes, estimating, measuring, [and the] reproduction of number patterns'.[25]

Literacy

There seems to be a relationship between literacy and numeracy,[26][27] which can be seen in young children. Depending on the level of literacy or numeracy at a young age, one can predict the growth of literacy and/ or numeracy skills in future development.[28] There is some evidence that humans may have an inborn sense of number. In one study for example, five-month-old infants were shown two dolls, which were then hidden with a screen. The babies saw the experimenter pull one doll from behind the screen. Without the child's knowledge, a second experimenter could remove, or add dolls, unseen behind the screen. When the screen was removed, the infants showed more surprise at an unexpected number (for example, if there were still two dolls). Some researchers have concluded that the babies were able to count, although others doubt this and claim the infants noticed surface area rather than number.[29]

Employment

Numeracy has a huge impact on employment.[30] In a work environment, numeracy can be a controlling factor affecting career achievements and failures.[30] Many professions require individuals to have well-developed numerical skills: for example, mathematician, physicist, accountant, actuary, Risk Analyst, financial analyst, engineer, and architect. This is why a major target of the Sustainable Development Goal 4 is to substantially increase the number of youths who have relevant skills for decent work and employment[31] because, even outside these specialized areas, the lack of numeracy skills can reduce employment opportunities and promotions, resulting in unskilled manual careers, low-paying jobs, and even unemployment.[32] For example, carpenters and interior designers need to be able to measure, use fractions, and handle budgets.[33] Another example of numeracy influencing employment was demonstrated at the Poynter Institute. The Poynter Institute has recently included numeracy as one of the skills required by competent journalists. Max Frankel, former executive editor of The New York Times, argues that "deploying numbers skillfully is as important to communication as deploying verbs". Unfortunately, it is evident that journalists often show poor numeracy skills. In a study by the Society of Professional Journalists, 58% of job applicants interviewed by broadcast news directors lacked an adequate understanding of statistical materials.[34]

To assess job applicants, psychometric numerical reasoning tests have been created by occupational psychologists, who are involved in the study of numeracy. These tests are used to assess ability to comprehend and apply numbers. They are sometimes administered with a time limit, so that the test-taker must think quickly and concisely. Research has shown that these tests are very useful in evaluating potential applicants because they do not allow the applicants to prepare for the test, unlike interview questions. This suggests that an applicant's results are reliable and accurate[citation needed]

These tests first became prevalent during the 1980s, following the pioneering work of psychologists, such as P. Kline, who published a book in 1986 entitled A handbook of test construction: Introduction to psychometric design, which explained that psychometric testing could provide reliable and objective results, which could be used to assess a candidate's numerical abilities.

Innumeracy and dyscalculia

The term innumeracy is a neologism, coined by analogy with illiteracy. Innumeracy refers to a lack of ability to reason with numbers. The term was coined by cognitive scientist Douglas Hofstadter; however, it was popularized in 1989 by mathematician John Allen Paulos in his book Innumeracy: Mathematical Illiteracy and its Consequences.

Developmental dyscalculia refers to a persistent and specific impairment of basic numerical-arithmetical skills learning in the context of normal intelligence.

Patterns and differences

The root causes of innumeracy vary. Innumeracy has been seen in those suffering from poor education and childhood deprivation of numeracy.[35] Innumeracy is apparent in children during the transition between numerical skills obtained before schooling and the new skills taught in the education departments because of their memory capacity to comprehend the material.[35] Patterns of innumeracy have also been observed depending on age, gender, and race.[36] Older adults have been associated with lower numeracy skills than younger adults.[36] Men have been identified to have higher numeracy skills than women.[30] Some studies seem to indicate young people of African heritage tend to have lower numeracy skills.[36] The Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) in which children at fourth-grade (average 10 to 11 years) and eighth-grade (average 14 to 15 years) from 49 countries were tested on mathematical comprehension. The assessment included tests for number, algebra (also called patterns and relationships at fourth grade), measurement, geometry, and data. The latest study, in 2003, found that children from Singapore at both grade levels had the highest performance. Countries like Hong Kong SAR, Japan, and Taiwan also shared high levels of numeracy. The lowest scores were found in countries like[clarification needed] South Africa, Ghana, and Saudi Arabia. Another finding showed a noticeable difference between boys and girls, with some exceptions. For example, girls performed significantly better in Singapore, and boys performed significantly better in the United States.[11]

Theory

There is a theory that innumeracy is more common than illiteracy when dividing cognitive abilities into two separate categories. David C. Geary, a notable cognitive developmental and evolutionary psychologist from the University of Missouri, created the terms "biological primary abilities" and "biological secondary abilities".[35] Biological primary abilities evolve over time and are necessary for survival. Such abilities include speaking a common language or knowledge of simple mathematics.[35] Biological secondary abilities are attained through personal experiences and cultural customs, such as reading or high level mathematics learned through schooling.[35] Literacy and numeracy are similar in the sense that they are both important skills used in life. However, they differ in the sorts of mental demands each makes. Literacy consists of acquiring vocabulary and grammatical sophistication, which seem to be more closely related to memorization, whereas numeracy involves manipulating concepts, such as in calculus or geometry, and builds from basic numeracy skills.[35] This could be a potential explanation of the challenge of being numerate.[35]

Innumeracy and risk perception in health decision-making

Health numeracy has been defined as "the degree to which individuals have the capacity to access, process, interpret, communicate, and act on numerical, quantitative, graphical, biostatistical, and probabilistic health information needed to make effective health decisions".[37] The concept of health numeracy is a component of the concept of health literacy. Health numeracy and health literacy can be thought of as the combination of skills needed for understanding risk and making good choices in health-related behavior.

Health numeracy requires basic numeracy but also more advanced analytical and statistical skills. For instance, health numeracy also requires the ability to understand probabilities or relative frequencies in various numerical and graphical formats, and to engage in Bayesian inference, while avoiding errors sometimes associated with Bayesian reasoning (see Base rate fallacy, Conservatism (Bayesian)). Health numeracy also requires understanding terms with definitions that are specific to the medical context. For instance, although 'survival' and 'mortality' are complementary in common usage, these terms are not complementary in medicine (see five-year survival rate).[38][39] Innumeracy is also a very common problem when dealing with risk perception in health-related behavior; it is associated with patients, physicians, journalists and policymakers.[36][39] Those who lack or have limited health numeracy skills run the risk of making poor health-related decisions because of an inaccurate perception of information.[20][40] For example, if a patient has been diagnosed with breast cancer, being innumerate may hinder her ability to comprehend her physician's recommendations, or even the severity of the health concern or even the likelihood of treatment benefits.[41] One study found that people tended to overestimate their chances of survival or even to choose lower-quality hospitals.[30] Innumeracy also makes it difficult or impossible for some patients to read medical graphs correctly.[42] Some authors have distinguished graph literacy from numeracy.[43] Indeed, many doctors exhibit innumeracy when attempting to explain a graph or statistics to a patient. A misunderstanding between a doctor and patient, due to either the doctor, patient, or both being unable to comprehend numbers effectively, could result in serious harm to health.

Different presentation formats of numerical information, for instance natural frequency icon arrays, have been evaluated to assist both low-numeracy and high-numeracy individuals.[36][44][45][46][47] Other data formats provide more assistance to low-numeracy people.[3][48][49]

Evolution of numeracy

In the field of economic history, numeracy is often used to assess human capital at times when there was no data on schooling or other educational measures. Using a method called age-heaping, researchers like Professor Jörg Baten study the development and inequalities of numeracy over time and throughout regions. For example, Baten[50] and Hippe find a numeracy gap between regions in western and central Europe and the rest of Europe for the period 1790–1880. At the same time, their data analysis reveals that these differences as well as within country inequality decreased over time. Taking a similar approach, Baten and Fourie[51] find overall high levels of numeracy for people in the Cape Colony (late 17th to early 19th century).

In contrast to these studies comparing numeracy over countries or regions, it is also possible to analyze numeracy within countries. For example, Baten, Crayen and Voth[52] look at the effects of war on numeracy in England, and Baten and Priwitzer[53] find a "military bias" in what is today western Hungary: people opting for a military career had - on average - better numeracy indicators (1 BCE to 3CE).

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Brooks, M.; Pui (2010). "Are individual differences in numeracy unique from general mental ability? A closer look at a common measure of numeracy". Individual Differences Research. 4. 8: 257–265.
  2. ^ Statistics Canada. "Building on our Competencies: Canadian Results of the International Adult Literacy and Skills Survey" (PDF). Statistics Canada. p. 209. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-09-27.
  3. ^ a b c Peters, Ellen (2020). Innumeracy in the wild: Misunderstanding and misusing numbers. Oxford University Press.
  4. ^ Gerardi, K.; Goette, L.; Meier, S. (2013). "Numerical ability predicts mortgage default". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (28): 11267–11271. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11011267G. doi:10.1073/pnas.1220568110. PMC 3710828. PMID 23798401.
  5. ^ Banks, J.; O'Dea, C.; Oldfield, Z. (2010). "Cognitive Function, Numeracy and Retirement Saving Trajectories*". The Economic Journal. 120 (548): F381–F410. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2010.02395.x. PMC 3249594. PMID 22228911.
  6. ^ a b c d Reyna, V.F.; Nelson, W.L.; Han, P.K.; Dieckmann, N.F. (2009). "How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (6): 943–973. doi:10.1037/a0017327. PMC 2844786. PMID 19883143.
  7. ^ a b Weller, J.A.; Dieckmann, N.F.; Tusler, M.; Mertz, C.K.; Burns, W.J.; Peters, E. (2013). "Development and Testing of an Abbreviated Numeracy Scale: A Rasch Analysis Approach". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 26 (2): 198–212. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.678.6236. doi:10.1002/bdm.1751. PMC 7161838. PMID 32313367.
  8. ^ Peters, E.; Tompkins, M.K.; Knoll, M.; Ardoin, S.P.; Shoots-Reinhard, B.; Meara, A.S. (2019). "Despite high objective numeracy, lower numeric confidence relates to worse financial and medical outcomes". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 116 (39): 19386–19391. Bibcode:2019PNAS..11619386P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1903126116. PMC 6765274. PMID 31501338.
  9. ^ Feigenson, Lisa; Dehaene, Stanislas; Spelke, Elizabeth (July 2004). "Core systems of number". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (7): 307–314. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002. PMID 15242690. S2CID 17313189.
  10. ^ Izard, V.; Sann, C.; Spelke, E.S.; Streri, A. (2009). "Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (25): 10382–10385. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10610382I. doi:10.1073/pnas.0812142106. PMC 2700913. PMID 19520833.
  11. ^ a b c d e f Dehaene, S.; Izard, V.; Spelke, E.; Pica, P. (2008). "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures". Science. 320 (5880): 1217–1220. Bibcode:2008Sci...320.1217D. doi:10.1126/science.1156540. PMC 2610411. PMID 18511690.
  12. ^ Nieder, A. (2005). "Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6 (3): 177–190. doi:10.1038/nrn1626. PMID 15711599. S2CID 14578049.
  13. ^ Halberda, J.; Mazzocco, M.L.M.M.; Feigenson, L. (2008). "Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement". Nature. 455 (7213): 665–668. Bibcode:2008Natur.455..665H. doi:10.1038/nature07246. PMID 18776888. S2CID 27196030.
  14. ^ Schwartz, L.M.; Woloshin, S.; Black, W.C.; Welch, H.G. (1997). "The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography". Annals of Internal Medicine. 127 (11): 966–972. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-127-11-199712010-00003. PMID 9412301. S2CID 19412405.
  15. ^ Lipkus, I.M.; Samsa, G.; Rimer, B.K. (2001). "General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples". Medical Decision Making. 21 (1): 37–44. doi:10.1177/0272989X0102100105. PMID 11206945. S2CID 25249366.
  16. ^ Cokely, E.T.; Galesic, M.; Schulz, E.; Ghazal, S.; Garcia-Retamero, R. (2012). "Measuring risk literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test" (PDF). Judgment and Decision Making. 7 (1): 25–47. doi:10.1017/S1930297500001819. S2CID 11617465.
  17. ^ Schapira, M.M.; Walker, C.M.; Cappaert, K.J.; Ganschow, P.S.; Fletcher, K.E.; McGinley, E.L.; Del Pozo, S.; Schauer, C.; Tarima, S.; Jacobs, E.A. (2012). "The Numeracy Understanding in Medicine Instrument: A Measure of Health Numeracy Developed Using Item Response Theory". Medical Decision Making. 32 (6): 851–865. doi:10.1177/0272989X12447239. PMC 4162626. PMID 22635285.
  18. ^ Fagerlin, A.; Zikmund-Fisher, B.J.; Ubel, P.A.; Jankovic, A.; Derry, H.A.; Smith, D.M. (2007). "Measuring Numeracy without a Math Test: Development of the Subjective Numeracy Scale". Medical Decision Making. 27 (5): 672–680. doi:10.1177/0272989X07304449. PMID 17641137. S2CID 30150256.
  19. ^ Osborn, Chandra Y.; Wallston, Kenneth A.; Shpigel, Adam; Cavanaugh, Kerri; Kripalani, Sunil; Rothman, Russell L. (June 2013). "Development and Validation of the General Health Numeracy Test (GHNT)". Patient Education and Counseling. 91 (3): 350–356. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2013.01.001. ISSN 0738-3991. PMC 3644342. PMID 23433635.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g h Ciampa, Philip J.; Osborn, Chandra Y.; Peterson, Neeraja B.; Rothman, Russell L. (13 December 2010). "Patient Numeracy, Perceptions of Provider Communication, and Colorectal Cancer Screening Utilization". Journal of Health Communication. 15 (sup3): 157–168. doi:10.1080/10810730.2010.522699. PMC 3075203. PMID 21154091.
  21. ^ Joerg, Baten; Mikołaj, Szołtysek; Monica, Campestrini (14 December 2016). "'Girl Power' in Eastern Europe? The human capital development of Central-Eastern and Eastern Europe in the seventeenth to nineteenth centuries and its determinants" (PDF). European Review of Economic History. doi:10.1093/ereh/hew017. S2CID 51963985. Archived from the original (PDF) on 28 February 2019.
  22. ^ a b c d e Melhuish, Edward C.; Phan, Mai B.; Sylva, Kathy; Sammons, Pam; Siraj-Blatchford, Iram; Taggart, Brenda (March 2008). "Effects of the Home Learning Environment and Preschool Center Experience upon Literacy and Numeracy Development in Early Primary School". Journal of Social Issues. 64 (1): 95–114. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x. S2CID 18460752.
  23. ^ Hughes, Martin (1986). Children and Number. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-13581-4.
  24. ^ Jeremy Kilpatrick; Jane Swafford; Bradford Findell (2001). Adding + it up: helping children learn mathematics. National Academy Press. ISBN 0-309-06995-5. OCLC 248386156.
  25. ^ Ewers-Rogers, Jennifer; Cowan, Richard (January 1996). "Children as Apprentices to Number". Early Child Development and Care. 125 (1): 15–25. doi:10.1080/0300443961250102. ISSN 0300-4430.
  26. ^ Bullock, James O. (October 1994), "Literacy in the Language of Mathematics", The American Mathematical Monthly, 101 (8): 735–743, doi:10.2307/2974528, JSTOR 2974528
  27. ^ Steen, Lynn Arthur (2001), "Mathematics and Numeracy: Two Literacies, One Language", The Mathematics Educator, 6 (1): 10–16
  28. ^ Purpura, David; Hume, L.; Sims, D.; Lonigan, C. (2011). "Early literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 110 (4): 647–658. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.004. PMID 21831396.
  29. ^ Numbers in Mind
  30. ^ a b c d Brooks, M.; Pui, S. (2010). "Are individual differences in numeracy unique from general mental ability? A closer look at a common measure of numeracy". Individual Differences Research. 4. 8: 257–265.
  31. ^ "SDG4's 10 targets". Global Campaign For Education. Retrieved 2020-09-22.
  32. ^ Ciampa, Philip J.; Osborn, Chandra Y.; Peterson, Neeraja B.; Rothman, Russell L. (2010). "Patient Numeracy, Perceptions of Provider Communication, and Colorectal Cancer Screening Utilization". Journal of Health Communication. 15 (Suppl 3): 157–168. doi:10.1080/10810730.2010.522699. PMC 3075203. PMID 21154091.
  33. ^ Melhuish, Edward C.; Phan, Mai B.; Sylva, Kathy; Sammons, Pam; Siraj-Blatchford, Iram; Taggart, Brenda (2008). "Effects of the Home Learning Environment and Preschool Center Experience upon Literacy and Numeracy Development in Early Primary School". Journal of Social Issues. 64 (1): 95–114. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x. S2CID 18460752.
  34. ^ Scanlan, Chip (2004). "Why Math Matters Archived 2009-01-14 at the Wayback Machine" Poynter Online, September 8, 2004.
  35. ^ a b c d e f g Lefevre, Jo-Anne (2000). "Research on the development of academic skills: Introduction to the special issue on early literacy and early numeracy". Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 54 (2): 57–60. doi:10.1037/h0088185. PMID 10881390.
  36. ^ a b c d e Donelle, L.; Hoffman-Goetz, L.; Arocha, J.F. (2007). "Assessing health numeracy among community-dwelling older adults". Journal of Health Communication. 7. 12 (7): 651–665. doi:10.1080/10810730701619919. PMID 17934942. S2CID 20421979.
  37. ^ Golbeck, AL; Ahlers-Schmidt, CR; Paschal, AM; Dismuke, SE (2005). "A definition and operational framework for health numeracy". American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 29 (4): 375–376. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2005.06.012. PMID 16242604.
  38. ^ Welch, H.G.; Schwartz, L.M.; Woloshin, S. (2000). "Are Increasing 5-Year Survival Rates Evidence of Success Against Cancer?". JAMA. 283 (22): 2975–2978. doi:10.1001/jama.283.22.2975. PMID 10865276.
  39. ^ a b Gigerenzer, G.; Gaissmaier, W.; Kurz-Milcke, E.; Schwartz, L.M.; Woloshin, S. (2007). "Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics". Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 8 (2): 53–96. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0025-7CFC-A. PMID 26161749.
  40. ^ Låg, T.; Bauger, L.; Lindberg, M.; Friborg, O. (2014). "The role of numeracy and intelligence in health-risk estimation and medical data interpretation". Behavioral Decision Making. 30 (2): 95–108. doi:10.1002/bdm.1788.
  41. ^ Lipkus, I.M.; Peters, E.; Kimmick, G.; Liotcheva, V.; Marcom, P. (2010). "Breast cancer patients' treatment expectations after exposure to the decision aid program, Adjuvant Online: The influence of numeracy". Medical Decision Making. 30 (4): 464–473. doi:10.1177/0272989X09360371. PMC 3616375. PMID 20160070.
  42. ^ Hess, R.; Visschers, V.H.M.; Siegrist, M.; Keller, C. (2011). "How do people perceive graphical risk communication? The role of subjective numeracy". Journal of Risk Research. 14: 47–61. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.488745. S2CID 146594087.
  43. ^ Galesic, M.; Garcia-Retamero, R. (2010). "Graph Literacy: A Cross-Cultural Comparison". Medical Decision Making. 31 (3): 444–457. doi:10.1177/0272989X10373805. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F299-8. PMID 20671213. S2CID 32662502.
  44. ^ Ancker, J.S.; Senathirajah, Y.; Kukafka, R.; Starren, J.B. (2006). "Design Features of Graphs in Health Risk Communication: A Systematic Review". Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association. 13 (6): 608–618. doi:10.1197/jamia.M2115. PMC 1656964. PMID 16929039.
  45. ^ Garcia-Retamero, R.; Okan, Y.; Cokely, E.T. (2012). "Using Visual Aids to Improve Communication of Risks about Health: A Review". The Scientific World Journal. 2012: 1–10. doi:10.1100/2012/562637. PMC 3354448. PMID 22629146.
  46. ^ Hoffrage, U.; Lindsey, S.; Hertwig, R.; Gigerenzer, G. (2000). "Medicine: Communicating Statistical Information". Science. 290 (5500): 2261–2262. doi:10.1126/science.290.5500.2261. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0025-9B18-3. PMID 11188724. S2CID 33050943.
  47. ^ Galesic, M.; Garcia-Retamero, R.; Gigerenzer, G. (2009). "Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks: Overcoming low numeracy". Health Psychology. 28 (2): 210–216. doi:10.1037/a0014474. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F6BE-3. PMID 19290713. S2CID 10545246.
  48. ^ Peters, E.; Dieckmann, N.; Dixon, A.; Hibbard, J.H.; Mertz, C.K. (2007). "Less is more in presenting quality information to consumers". Medical Care Research & Review. 64 (2): 169–190. doi:10.1177/10775587070640020301. PMID 17406019. S2CID 1001326.
  49. ^ Peters, E.; Dieckmann, N.; Våstfjåll, D.; Mertz, C.K.; Slovic, P.; Hibbard, J.H. (2009). "Bringing meaning to numbers: The impact of evaluative categories on decisions". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 15 (3): 213–227. doi:10.1037/a0016978. PMID 19751072. S2CID 11035873.
  50. ^ Baten, Jörg; Hippe, Ralph (2012). "The early regional development of human capital in Europe, 1790–1880" (PDF). Scandinavian Economic History Review. 60 (3): 254–289. doi:10.1080/03585522.2012.727763. S2CID 154669586.
  51. ^ Baten, Jörg; Fourie, Johan (2015). "Numeracy of Africans, Asians, and Europeans during the early modern period: new evidence from Cape Colony court registers". The Economic History Review. 68 (2): 632–656. doi:10.1111/1468-0289.12064. hdl:10.1111/1468-0289.12064. S2CID 51961313.
  52. ^ Baten, Jörg; Crayen, Dorothee; Voth, Hans-Joachim (2014). "Numeracy and the impact of high food prices in industrializing Britain, 1780–1850" (PDF). Review of Economics and Statistics. 96 (3): 418–430. doi:10.1162/REST_a_00403. S2CID 3518364.
  53. ^ Baten, Jörg; Priwitzer, Stefan (2015). "Social and intertemporal differences of basic numeracy in Pannonia (first century BCE to third century CE)". Scandinavian Economic History Review. 63 (2): 110–134. doi:10.1080/03585522.2015.1032339. S2CID 51962193.

Read other articles:

Franz BöhmeFranz BöhmeLahir15 April 1885Zeltweg, Kadipaten Steiermark, Austria-Hungaria kini AustriaMeninggal29 Mei 1947(1947-05-29) (umur 62)Nuremberg, Bayern, Wilayah pendudukan JermanDikebumikanSt. Leonhard-Friedhof, Graz, AustriaPengabdian Austria-Hungaria (hingga tahun 1918) Republik Austria Pertama (hingga tahun 1938) Jerman NaziDinas/cabangAngkatan Darat Austria-HungariaBundesheerWehrmachtLama dinas1900–1938 (Austria)1938–1945 (Jerman)PangkatGeneralmajor (Austria)G...

 

 

Verrone commune di Italia Tempat categoria:Articles mancats de coordenades Negara berdaulatItaliaRegion di ItaliaPiedmontProvinsi di ItaliaProvinsi Biella NegaraItalia Ibu kotaVerrone PendudukTotal1.166  (2023 )GeografiLuas wilayah8,59 km² [convert: unit tak dikenal]Ketinggian277 m Berbatasan denganCandelo Gaglianico Massazza Salussola Benna Cerrione Sandigliano Informasi tambahanKode pos13871 Zona waktuUTC+1 UTC+2 Kode telepon015 ID ISTAT096076 Kode kadaster ItaliaL785 Lain-lainSi...

 

 

Disambiguazione – Se stai cercando altri significati, vedi Serie A 1938-1939 (disambigua). Serie A 1938-1939 Competizione Serie A Sport Calcio Edizione 39ª (10ª di Serie A) Organizzatore Direttorio Divisioni Superiori Date dal 18 settembre 1938al 28 maggio 1939 Luogo  Italia Partecipanti 16 Formula girone unico Risultati Vincitore Bologna(5º titolo) Retrocessioni LivornoLucchese Statistiche Miglior marcatore Aldo Boffi Héctor Puricelli(19 ex aequo) Incontri dispu...

Architectural order Illustration of the Composite order, made in 1695 and kept in Deutsche Fotothek The Composite order is a mixed order, combining the volutes of the Ionic order capital with the acanthus leaves of the Corinthian order.[1] In many versions the composite order volutes are larger, however, and there is generally some ornament placed centrally between the volutes. The column of the composite order is typically ten diameters high, though as with all the orders these detai...

 

 

Logo MYTV Halaman ini memuat daftar acara yang ditayangkan MYTV.[1] Acara saat ini Kosong Acara yang pernah ditayangkan Banten TV Banten Siang Banten Hari Ini Berani Dangdut Teras Keluarga Godain (Goyang Dangdut Indonesia) Ngobrol Sareng Kang Ustadz INTV Seputar Kita Seputar Kita Fokus IN Business IN Sport Sensi (Sensasi Selebritis) Classic Zone Countdown IN Music K-Pop Zone Kidung Rohani Senandung Religi Amanda Memori Cinta Soraya Law & Order Ledek (Lenong Demokrasi) Curahan Qol...

 

 

Typical aspect of the bread after being toasted, soaked and brushed Torricado is a culinary specialty from the Ribatejo region of Portugal, consisting of pieces of bread toasted over charcoal, soaked in olive oil and brushed with garlic and salt.[1] It is served mainly as a side dish to grilled salt cod, but also to sardines[2] and pork.[3] The dish is common both to the Tagus riverside plains and the hill rugged area of the vineyards, mainly in the area of Almeirim, A...

PausLeo VIIIAwal masa kepausan964Akhir masa kepausanFebruari 965PendahuluYohanes XIIPenerusBenediktus VInformasi pribadiNama lahirtidak diketahuiLahirtanggal tidak diketahuiRoma, ItaliaWafatFebruari 965Roma, ItaliaPaus lainnya yang bernama Leo Paus Leo VIII (???-Februari 965) adalah Paus Gereja Katolik Roma sejak 964 hingga Februari 965. Ia adalah seorang Antipaus dari tahun 963 hingga 964. Didahului oleh:Yohanes XII Paus964 – 965 Diteruskan oleh:Benediktus V Artikel bertopik Paus dala...

 

 

Gearing-class destroyer For other ships with the same name, see USS Rowan. USS Rowan underway in 1965 History United States NameRowan NamesakeStephen C. Rowan BuilderTodd Pacific Shipyards Laid down25 March 1944 Launched29 December 1944 Sponsored byMrs. David S. Folsom Commissioned31 March 1945 Decommissioned18 December 1975 Stricken30 January 1976 Identification Callsign: NTFJ Hull number: DD-782 MottoIf It's There, We'll Be There Nickname(s)Black Bird Honors andawardsSee Awards FateTransfer...

 

 

Chemical compound and gastrointestinal medication SucralfateClinical dataTrade namesCarafateAHFS/Drugs.comMonographMedlinePlusa681049License data US DailyMed: Sucralfate Routes ofadministrationBy mouth, rectalATC codeA02BX02 (WHO) Legal statusLegal status US: ℞-only[1] In general: ℞ (Prescription only) Pharmacokinetic dataBioavailability3-5% (local acting)MetabolismGI; liver: unknownElimination half-lifeunknownExcretionFeces, urineIdentifiers IUP...

Taiwanese Corporation For sinology (Chinese studies), see Sinology. For the study of sound, see Sonology. This article relies excessively on references to primary sources. Please improve this article by adding secondary or tertiary sources. Find sources: Synology – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (January 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this message) Synology, Inc.Native name群暉科技股份有限公司Company typePrivateIndustryNetwo...

 

 

Darr: A Violent Love StoryडरڈرPosterSutradaraYash ChopraProduserYash ChopraDitulis olehHoney IraniJaved SiddiquiPemeranSunny DeolShahrukh Khan Juhi ChawlaAnupam KherDilip TahilPenata musikShiv-HariSinematograferManmohan SinghPenyuntingKeshav NaiduDistributorYash Raj FilmsTanggal rilis 24 Desember 1993 (1993-12-24) Durasi177 minutesNegaraIndiaBahasaHindiPendapatankotor₹20 crore (setara dengan ₹103 crore atau US$14 juta pada tahun 2023)[1] Darr: A ...

 

 

Human settlement in EnglandWalton-on-TrentSt Laurence Church, Walton-on-TrentWalton-on-TrentLocation within DerbyshirePopulation872 (2011)(census) or 891 (2018)(estimate)OS grid referenceSK216180Civil parishWalton upon TrentDistrictSouth DerbyshireShire countyDerbyshireRegionEast MidlandsCountryEnglandSovereign stateUnited KingdomPost townSWADLINCOTEPostcode districtDE12PoliceDerbyshireFireDerbyshireAmbulanceEast Midlands List of places UK England Derbyshire 52�...

Частина серії проФілософіяLeft to right: Plato, Kant, Nietzsche, Buddha, Confucius, AverroesПлатонКантНіцшеБуддаКонфуційАверроес Філософи Епістемологи Естетики Етики Логіки Метафізики Соціально-політичні філософи Традиції Аналітична Арістотелівська Африканська Близькосхідна іранська Буддій�...

 

 

Chinese painting style using black ink Ink wash paintingLiang Kai (Chinese: 梁楷, 1140–1210), Drunken Celestial (Chinese: 潑墨仙人), ink on Xuan paper, 12th century, Southern Song (Chinese), National Palace Museum, TaipeiChinese nameTraditional Chinese水墨畫Simplified Chinese水墨画TranscriptionsStandard MandarinHanyu PinyinshuǐmòhuàKorean nameHangul수묵화Hanja水墨畵TranscriptionsRevised RomanizationsumukhwaMcCune–ReischauersumukhwaJapanese nameKanji1. 水...

 

 

Codeks Gigas; halaman pengantar yang menampilkan ilustrasi sosok setan Codex Gigas (bahasa Inggris: Giant Book) adalah manuskrip abad pertengahan terbesar di dunia.[1] Codex ini juga dikenal sebagai Kitab Suci Setan karena ilustrasi dan legenda penciptaan setan pada codex tersebut. Codex Gigas diperkirakan dibuat pada awal abad ke-13 di biara Podlažice, Bohemia (kini Republik Ceko). Pada manuskrip ini terdapat Kitab Suci Vulgata, dan juga dokumen-dokumen historis yang ditulis dal...

Laut ArgentinaLaut Argentina, Lepas pantai Atlantik ArgentinaLetakPantai Timur ArgentinaBagian dariSamudra Atlantik SelatanAsal sungaiRío de la Plata, Sungai Colorado, Río Negro, Sungai Chubut, Sungai DeseadoTerletak di negaraArgentina, Chile, Uruguay, Falkland Islands (UK)Area permukaan1.000.000 kilometer persegi (390.000 sq mi)KepulauanFalkland Islands, Tierra del FuegoLihat peta yang diperkecilLihat peta yang diperbesar Laut Argentina (Spanyol: Mar Argentino) adalah nama yang d...

 

 

Artikel ini bukan mengenai Wang Xinyu. Wang Xiyu王曦雨Wang, 2022Kebangsaan TiongkokLahir28 Maret 2001 (umur 23)Taixing, ChinaTinggi182 m (597 ft 1 in)Tipe pemainLeft-handed (two-handed backhand)Total hadiahUS $959,895TunggalRekor (M–K)159–102 (60.92%)Gelar3 ITFPeringkat tertinggiNo. 50 (7 November 2022)Peringkat saat iniNo. 50 (7 November 2022)Hasil terbaik di Grand Slam (tunggal)Australia Terbuka2R (2022)Prancis Terbuka1R (2021)Wimbledon1R (2022)AS Terbuk...

 

 

Spanish Basque officer In this Spanish name, the first or paternal surname is Zumalacárregui and the second or maternal family name is Imaz. Tomás de ZumalacárreguiLithography of Zumalacárregui, 1837Captain general of the Carlist ArmyIn office23 June 1835 – 24 June 1835MonarchInfante Carlos Personal detailsBornTomás de Zumalacárregui e Imaz(1788-12-29)29 December 1788Ormaiztegi, Gipuzkoa, SpainDied24 June 1835(1835-06-24) (aged 46)ParentsFrancisco Antonio de Zuma...

Contrast agent for imaging, no longer used clinically Thorotrast bottle Thorotrast is a suspension containing particles of the radioactive compound thorium dioxide, ThO2; it was used as a radiocontrast agent in clinical radiography in the 1930s to 1950s. It is no longer used clinically. Thorium compounds produce excellent images because of thorium's high opacity to X-rays (it has a high cross section for absorption). However, thorium is retained in the body, and it is radioactive, emitting ha...

 

 

横浜F・マリノス > トリコロールマーメイズ この記事には複数の問題があります。改善やノートページでの議論にご協力ください。 出典がまったく示されていないか不十分です。内容に関する文献や情報源が必要です。(2022年9月) 組織の特筆性の基準を満たしていないおそれがあります。(2022年9月)出典検索?: トリコロールマーメイズ – ニュース&#...