Negative affectivity

In psychology, negative affectivity (NA), or negative affect, is a personality variable that involves the experience of negative emotions and poor self-concept.[1] Negative affectivity subsumes a variety of negative emotions, including anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, fear,[2] and nervousness. Low negative affectivity is characterized by frequent states of calmness and serenity, along with states of confidence, activeness, and great enthusiasm.

Individuals differ in negative emotional reactivity.[3] Trait negative affectivity roughly corresponds to the dominant personality factor of anxiety/neuroticism that is found within the Big Five personality traits as emotional stability.[4] The Big Five are characterized as openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism. Neuroticism can plague an individual with severe mood swings, frequent sadness, worry, and being easily disturbed,[1][3] and predicts the development and onset of all "common" mental disorders.[5] Research shows that negative affectivity relates to different classes of variables: Self-reported stress and (poor) coping skills,[1][6][7] health complaints, and frequency of unpleasant events.[8] Weight gain and mental health complaints are often experienced as well.

People who express high negative affectivity view themselves and a variety of aspects of the world around them in generally negative terms.[1] Negative affectivity is strongly related to life satisfaction. Individuals high in negative affect will exhibit, on average, higher levels of distress, anxiety, and dissatisfaction, and tend to focus on the unpleasant aspects of themselves, the world, the future, and other people, and also evoke more negative life events.[8] The similarities between these affective traits and life satisfaction have led some researchers to view both positive and negative affect with life satisfaction as specific indicators of the broader construct of subjective well-being.

Negative affect arousal mechanisms can induce negative affective states as evidenced by a study conducted by Stanley S. Seidner on negative arousal and white noise. The study quantified reactions from Mexican and Puerto Rican participants in response to the devaluation of speakers from other ethnic origins.[9]

Measurement

There are many instruments that can be used to measure negative affectivity, including measures of related concepts, such as neuroticism and trait anxiety.[10] Two frequently used are:

PANAS The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule incorporates a 10-item negative affect scale.[11] The PANAS-X is an expanded version of PANAS that incorporates negative affect subscales for Fear, Sadness, Guilt, Hostility, and Shyness.

I-PANAS-SF – The International Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short Form is an extensively validated brief, cross-culturally reliable 10-item version of the PANAS.[12] Negative Affect items are Afraid, Ashamed, Hostile, Nervous and Upset. Internal consistency reliabilities between .72 and .76 are reported. The I-PANAS-SF was developed to eliminate redundant and ambiguous items and thereby derive an efficient measure for general use in research situations where either time or space are limited, or where international populations are of interest but where English may not be the mother tongue.[12]

Benefits

Studies have indicated that negative affect has important, beneficial impacts on cognition and behavior. These developments were a departure from earlier psychological research, which was characterized by a unilateral emphasis on the benefits of positive affect.[13] Both states of affect influence mental processes and behavior.[14]

Benefits of negative affect are present in areas of cognition including perception, judgement, memory and interpersonal personal relations.[13][14] Since negative affect relies more on cautious processing than preexisting knowledge, people with negative affect tend to perform better in instances involving deception, manipulation, impression formation, and stereotyping. Negative affectivity's analytical and detailed processing of information leads to fewer reconstructive-memory errors, whereas positive mood relies on broader schematic to thematic information that ignores detail.[15] Thus, information processing in negative moods reduces the misinformation effect and increases overall accuracy of details.[13] People also exhibit less interfering responses to stimuli when given descriptions or performing any cognitive task.[13]

Judgment

People are notoriously susceptible to forming inaccurate judgments based on biases and limited information. Evolutionary theories propose that negative affective states tend to increase skepticism and decrease reliance on preexisting knowledge.[16] Consequently, judgmental accuracy is improved in areas such as impression formation, reducing fundamental attribution error, stereotyping, and gullibility.[13] While sadness is normally associated with the hippocampus, it does not produce the same side effects that would be associated with feelings of pleasure or excitement. Sadness correlates with feeling blue or the creation of tears, while excitement may cause a spike in blood pressure and one's pulse. As far as judgment goes, most people think about how they themselves feel about a certain situation. They will jump right to their current mood when asked a question. However, some mistake this process when using their current mood to justify a reaction to a stimulus. If they are only a little sad, their reactions and input may be negative as a whole.

Impression formation

First impressions are one of the most basic forms of judgments people make on a daily basis; yet judgment formation is a complex and fallible process. Negative affect is shown to decrease errors in forming impressions based on presuppositions.[13] One common judgment error is the halo effect, or the tendency to form unfounded impressions of people based on known but irrelevant information.[13] For instance, more attractive people are often attributed with more positive qualities. Research demonstrates that positive affect tends to increase the halo effect, whereas negative affect decreases it.[17]

A study involving undergraduate students demonstrated a halo effect in identifying a middle-aged man as more likely to be a philosopher than an unconventional, young woman.[17] These halo effects were nearly eliminated when participants were in a negative affective state. In the study, researchers sorted participants into either happy or sad groups using an autobiographical mood induction task in which participants reminisced on sad or happy memories.[17] Then, participants read a philosophical essay by a fake academic who was identified as either a middle-aged, bespectacled man or as a young, unorthodox-looking woman. The fake writer was evaluated on intelligence and competence. The positive affect group exhibited a strong halo effect, rating the male writer significantly higher than the female writer in competence.[17] The negative affect group exhibited almost no halo effects rating the two equally. Researchers concluded that impression formation is improved by negative affect.[17] Their findings support theories that negative affect results in more elaborate processing based upon external, available information.[17]

Fundamental attribution error

The systematic, attentive approach caused by negative affect reduces fundamental attribution error, the tendency to inaccurately attribute behavior to a person's internal character without taking external, situational factors into account.[18][19] The fundamental attribution error (FAE) is connected with positive affect since it occurs when people use top-down cognitive processing based on inferences. Negative affect stimulates bottom-up, systematic analysis that reduces fundamental attribution error.[18]

This effect is documented in FAE research in which students evaluated a fake debater on attitude and likability based on an essay the "debater" wrote.[18] After being sorted into positive or negative affect groups, participants read one of two possible essays arguing for one side or another on a highly controversial topic. Participants were informed that the debater was assigned a stance to take in the essay that did not necessarily reflect his views.[18] Still, the positive affect groups rated debaters who argued unpopular views as holding the same attitude expressed in the essay. They were also rated as unlikeable compared to debaters with popular stances, thus, demonstrating FAE. In contrast, the data for the negative affect group displayed no significant difference in ratings for debaters with popular stance and debaters with unpopular stances.[18] These results indicate that positive affect assimilation styles promote fundamental attribution error, and negative affect accommodation styles minimize the error in respect to judging people.[13]

Stereotyping

Negative affect benefits judgment in diminishing the implicit use of stereotypes by promoting closer attention to stimuli.[13] In one study, participants were less likely to discriminate against targets that appeared Muslim when in a negative affective state.[20] After organizing participants into positive and negative affect groups, researchers had them play a computer game. Participants had to make rapid decisions to shoot only at targets carrying a gun.[20] Some of the targets wore turbans making them appear Muslim. As expected, there was a significant bias against Muslim targets resulting in a tendency to shoot at them.[13] However, this tendency decreased with subjects in negative affective states. Positive affect groups developed more aggressive tendencies toward Muslims.[20] Researchers concluded that negative affect leads to less reliance on internal stereotypes, thus decreasing judgmental bias.[13]

Gullibility

Multiple studies have shown that negative affectivity has a beneficial role in increasing skepticism and decreasing gullibility.[13] Because negative affective states increase external analysis and attention to details, people in negative states are better able to detect deception.[13][16]

Researchers have presented findings in which students in negative affective states had improved lie detection compared to students in positive affective states.[21] In a study, students watched video clips of everyday people either lying or telling the truth. First, music was used to induce positive, negative, or neutral affect in participants.[21] Then, experimenters played 14 video messages that had to be identified by participants as true or false. As expected, the negative affect group performed better in veracity judgments than the positive affect group who performed no better than chance.[21] Researchers believe that the negative affect groups detected deception more successfully because they attended to stimulus details and systematically built inferences from those details.[21]

Memory

Memory has been found to have many failures that affect the accuracy of recalled memories.[22] This has been especially pragmatic in criminal settings as eyewitness memories have been found to be less reliable than one would hope. However, the externally focused and accommodative processing of negative affect has a positive effect on the overall improvement of memory. This is evidenced by reduction of the misinformation effect, and the number of false memories reported.[13] The knowledge implies that negative affect can be used to enhance eyewitness memory;[23] however, additional research suggests that the extent to which memory is improved by negative affect does not sufficiently improve eyewitness testimonies to significantly reduce its error.

Misinformation effect

Negative affect has been shown to decrease susceptibility of incorporating misleading information, which is related to the misinformation effect.[13] The misinformation effect refers to the finding that misleading information presented between the encoding of an event and its subsequent recall influences a witness's memory.[22] This corresponds to two types of memory failure:

Suggestibility: When recollections are influenced by the prodding or expectations of others creating false memories.[22][24]
Misattribution: When a witness gets confused and misattributes the misinformation to the original event. Also defined as the retroactive interference: When later information interferes with the ability to retain previously encoded information.[22]
In witness of events

Negative mood is shown to decrease suggestibility error. This is seen through reduced amounts of incorporation of false memories when misleading information is present. On the other hand, positive affect has shown to increase susceptibility to misleading information. An experiment with undergraduate students supported these results. Participants began the study in a lecture hall and witnessed what they thought was an unexpected five-minute belligerent encounter between an intruder and the lecturer. A week later, these participants watched a 10-minute-long video that generated either a positive, negative or neutral mood. They then completed a brief questionnaire about the previous incident between the intruder and lecturer that they witnessed the week earlier. In this questionnaire half of the participants received questions with misleading information and the other half received questions without any misleading information. This manipulation was used to determine if participants were susceptible to suggestibility failure. After 45 minutes of unrelated distractors participants were given a set of true or false questions which tested for false memories. Participants experiencing negative moods reported fewer numbers of false memories, whereas those experiencing positive moods reported a greater amount of false memories. This implies that positive affect promotes integration of misleading details and negative affect reduces the misinformation effect.[25][26]

In recall of past public events

People who experience negative affectivity following an event report fewer reconstructive false memories. This was evidenced by two studies conducted around public events. The first surrounded the events of the televised O.J. Simpson trial. Participants were asked to fill out questionnaires three times: one week, two months and a year after the televised verdict. These questionnaires measured participant emotion towards the verdict and the accuracy of their recalled memory of what occurred during the trial. Overall the study found that although participant response to the event outcome did not affect the quantity of remembered information, it did influence the likelihood of false memory. Participants who were pleased with the verdict of the O.J. Simpson trial were more likely to falsely believe something occurred during the trial than those who were displeased with the verdict.[27][28] Another experiment found the same findings with Red Sox fans and Yankees fans in their overall memory of events that occurred in the final game of a 2004 playoff series in which the Red Sox defeated the Yankees.[28][29] The study found that the Yankees fans had better memory of events that occurred than the Red Sox fans. The results from both of these experiments are consistent with the findings that negative emotion can lead to fewer memory errors and thus increased memory accuracy of events.[28]

Degree of enhanced memory

Although negative affect has been shown to decrease the misinformation effect, the degree to which memory is improved is not enough to make a significant effect on witness testimony. In fact, emotions, including negative affect, are shown to reduce accuracy in identifying perpetrators from photographic lineups. Researchers demonstrated this effect in an experiment in which participants watched a video that induced either negative emotion or a neutral mood. The two videos were deliberately similar except for the action of interest, which was either a mugging (negative emotion) or a conversation (neutral emotion). After watching one of the two videos participants are shown perpetrator lineups, which either contained the target perpetrator from the video or a foil, a person that looked similar to the target. The results revealed that the participants who watched the emotion-induced video were more likely to incorrectly identify the innocent foil than to correctly identify the perpetrator. Neutral participants were more likely to correctly identify the perpetrator in comparison to their emotional counterparts. This demonstrates that emotional affect in forensic settings decreases accuracy of eyewitness memory.[30] These findings are consistent with prior knowledge that stress and emotion greatly impair eyewitness ability to recognitive perpetrators.[22]

Interpersonal benefits

Negative affectivity can produce several interpersonal benefits. It can cause subjects to be more polite and considerate with others. Unlike positive mood, which causes less assertive approaches, negative affectivity can, in many ways, cause a person to be more polite and elaborate when making requests.[13]

Negative affectivity increases the accuracy of social perceptions and inferences. Specifically, high negative-affectivity people have more negative, but accurate, perceptions of the impression they make to others. People with low negative affectivity form overly-positive, potentially inaccurate impression of others that can lead to misplaced trust.[31]

Intergroup discrimination

A research conducted by Forgas J.P studied how affectivity can influence intergroup discrimination. He measured affectivity by how people allocate rewards to in-group and out-group members. In the procedure, participants had to describe their interpretations after looking at patterns of judgments about people. Afterwards, participants were exposed to a mood induction process, where they had to watch videotapes designed to elicit negative or positive affectivity. Results showed that participants with positive affectivity were more negative and discriminated more than participants with negative affectivity. Also, happy participants were more likely to discriminate between in-group and out-group members than sad participants.[32] Negative affect is often associated with team selection. It is viewed as a trait that could make selecting individuals for a team irrelevant, thus preventing knowledge from becoming known or predicted for current issues that may arise.

Communication

Negative affectivity subconsciously signals a challenging social environment. Negative mood may increase a tendency to conform to social norms.[1]

In a study, college students were exposed to a mood induction process. After the mood induction process, participants were required to watch a show with positive and negative elements. After watching the show, they were asked to engage on a hypothetical conversation in which they "describe the episode (they) just observed to a friend". Their speech was recorded and transcribed during this task. Results showed that speakers in a negative mood had a better quality descriptions and greater amount of information and details. These results show that negative mood can improve people's communication skills.[2]

A negative mood is closely linked to better conversation because it makes use of the hippocampus and different regions of the brain.[citation needed] When someone is upset, that individual may see or hear things differently than an individual who is very upbeat and happy all the time. The small details the negative individual picks up may be something completely overlooked before. Anxiety disorders are often associated with over-thinking and ruminating on topics that would seem irrelevant and pointless to an individual without a disorder. OCD is one common anxiety trait that allows the affected individual a different insight on how things may appear to be. A person that makes use of his or her negative affect has a different view of the world and what goes on in it, thus making their conversations different and interesting to others.

Self-disclosure

Results of one study show that participants with negative affectivity were more careful with the information they shared with others, being more cautious with who they could trust or not. Researchers found that negative mood not only decreases intimacy levels but also increases caution in placing trust in others.[33]

Enhanced ability to experience feelings

Negative affect is regularly recognized as a "stable, heritable trait tendency to experience a broad range of negative feelings, such as worry, anxiety, self-criticisms, and a negative self-view". This allows one to feel every type of emotion, which is regarded as a normal part of life and human nature. So, while the emotions themselves are viewed as negative, the individual experiencing them should not be classified as a negative person or depressed. They are going through a normal process and are feeling something that many individuals may not be able to feel or process due to differing problems.

Fit with evolutionary psychology

These findings complement evolutionary psychology theories that affective states serve adaptive functions in promoting suitable cognitive strategies to deal with environmental challenges.[14] Positive affect is associated with assimilative, top-down processing used in response to familiar, benign environments. Negative affect is connected with accommodative, bottom-up processing in response to unfamiliar, or problematic environments.[16] Thus, positive affectivity promotes simplistic heuristic approaches that rely on preexisting knowledge and assumptions. Conversely, negative affectivity promotes controlled, analytic approaches that rely on externally drawn information.[16]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e Watson, D.; Clark, L. A. (1984). "Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience negative aversive emotional states". Psychological Bulletin. 96 (3): 465–490. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.465. PMID 6393179.
  2. ^ a b Koch, Alex S.; Forgas, Joseph P.; Matovic, Diana (August 2013). "Can negative mood improve your conversation? Affective influences on conforming to Grice's communication norms". European Journal of Social Psychology. 43 (5): 326–334. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1950.
  3. ^ a b Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the Anxiety disorders, (pp. 681-706), Hilssdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  4. ^ Jeronimus, B.F.; Ormel, J.; Aleman, A.; Penninx, B.W.J.H.; Riese, H. (2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID 23410535. S2CID 43717734.
  5. ^ Jeronimus, B.F.; Kotov, R.; Riese, H.; Ormel, J. (2016). "Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants". Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 1–24. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID 27523506. S2CID 23548727.
  6. ^ Tessler, R.; Mechanic, D. (1978). "Psychological distress and perceived health status". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 19 (3): 254–262. doi:10.2307/2136558. JSTOR 2136558. PMID 701774.
  7. ^ Wills, T. A. (1986). "Stress and coping in early adolescence: Relationships to substance use in urban school samples". Health Psychology. 5 (6): 503–529. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.5.6.503. PMID 3492372.
  8. ^ a b Jeronimus, B. F.; Riese, H.; Sanderman, R.; Ormel, J. (2014). "Mutual Reinforcement Between Neuroticism and Life Experiences: A Five-Wave, 16-Year Study to Test Reciprocal Causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
  9. ^ Seidner, Stanley S. (1991), Negative Affect Arousal Reactions from Mexican and Puerto Rican Respondents, Washington, D.C. ERIC ED346711
  10. ^ Watson, D.; Clark, L. A. (1984). "Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states". Psychological Bulletin. 96 (3): 465–490. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.465. PMID 6393179.
  11. ^ Watson, D.; Clark, L. A.; Tellegen, A. (1988). "Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 54 (6): 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. PMID 3397865. S2CID 7679194.
  12. ^ a b Thompson, E.R. (2007). "Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)" (PDF). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 38 (2): 227–242. doi:10.1177/0022022106297301. S2CID 145498269.[permanent dead link]
  13. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Forgas, Joseph (2013). "Don't Worry, Be Sad! On the Cognitive, Motivational, and Interpersonal Benefits of Negative Mood". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 22 (3): 225–232. doi:10.1177/0963721412474458. S2CID 55629116.
  14. ^ a b c Forgas, Joseph (30 January 2010). "Cognitive Theories of Affect". Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. pp. 1–3. doi:10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy2003. ISBN 9780470479216.
  15. ^ Chaiken, Shelly; Trope, Yaacov, eds. (1999). Dual-process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-57230-421-5. OCLC 40489291. Retrieved 15 February 2022.
  16. ^ a b c d Forgas, Joseph; Herbert Bless (2006). Klaus Fiedler; Joseph Forgas (eds.). Affect in social thinking and behavior (12 ed.). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. pp. 65–84. ISBN 978-1-84169-454-2.
  17. ^ a b c d e f Forgas, Joseph (2011). "She just doesn't look like a philosopher…? Affective influences on the halo effect in impression formatio". European Journal of Social Psychology. 41 (7): 812–817. doi:10.1002/ejsp.842.
  18. ^ a b c d e Forgas, Joseph (1998). "On being happy and mistaken: Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 75 (2): 318–331. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.318. PMID 9731311.
  19. ^ Forgas, Joseph (2011). "She just doesn't look like a philosopher…? Affective influences on the halo effect in impression formation". European Journal of Social Psychology. 41 (7): 812–817. doi:10.1002/ejsp.842.
  20. ^ a b c Unkelbach, Christian; Joseph P. Forgas; Thomas F. Denson (2008). "The turban effect: The influence of Muslim headgear and induced affect on aggressive responses in the shooter bias paradigm". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44 (5): 1409–1413. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.003.
  21. ^ a b c d Reinhard, Marc-André; Norbert Schwarz (2012). "The Influence of Affective States on the Process of Lie Detection". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 18 (4): 377–389. doi:10.1037/a0030466. PMID 23148455.
  22. ^ a b c d e Robinson-Riegler, Bridget Robinson-Riegler, Gregory (2012). Cognitive psychology: Applying the Science of the mind (3rd ed.). Boston: Pearson Allyn & Bacon. ISBN 978-0-205-03364-5.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Joseph P. Forgas; Klaus Fiedler; Constantine Sedikides, eds. (2013). "The upside of feeling down: The benefits of negative mood for social cognition and social behavior". Social thinking and interpersonal behavior (PDF). New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 221–238. ISBN 978-0-203-13967-7.
  24. ^ LaPaglia, Jessica A.; Wilford, Miko M.; Rivard, Jillian R.; Chan, Jason C. K.; Fisher, Ronald P. (October 2013). "Misleading Suggestions can Alter Later Memory Reports even Following a Cognitive Interview". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 28: 1–9. doi:10.1002/acp.2950.
  25. ^ Forgas, Joseph P.; Laham, Simon M.; Vargas, Patrick T. (November 2005). "Mood effects on eyewitness memory: Affective influences on susceptibility to misinformation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 41 (6): 574–588. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.11.005.
  26. ^ Weiner, Irving B., ed. (2012). "Affective Influences on Cognition". Handbook of psychology (Web) (2nd ed.). Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley. ISBN 9781118133880.
  27. ^ Levine, Linda; Bluck, Susan (June 2004). "Painting with broad strokes: Happiness and the malleability of event memory". Cognition & Emotion. 18 (4): 559–574. doi:10.1080/02699930341000446. S2CID 144508535.
  28. ^ a b c Kensinger, Elizabeth A. (August 2007). "Negative Emotion Enhances Memory Accuracy: Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (4): 213–218. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x. S2CID 16885166.
  29. ^ Kensinger, Elizabeth A.; Schacter, Daniel L. (October 2006). "When the Red Sox shocked the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13 (5): 757–763. doi:10.3758/BF03193993. PMID 17328369.
  30. ^ Houston, Kate A.; Clifford, Brian R.; Phillips, Louise H.; Memon, Amina (2013). "The emotional eyewitness: The effects of emotion on specific aspects of eyewitness recall and recognition performance". Emotion. 13 (1): 118–128. doi:10.1037/a0029220. PMID 22775133.
  31. ^ Campbell, Jennifer D.; Fehr, Beverley (1990). "Self-esteem and perceptions of conveyed impressions: Is negative affectivity associated with greater realism?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (1): 122–133. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.1.122. PMID 2308069.
  32. ^ Forgas, Joseph P.; Fiedler, Klaus (1996). "Us and them: Mood effects on intergroup discrimination". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 70 (1): 28–40. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.28.
  33. ^ Forgas, Joseph P. (2011). "Affective influences on self-disclosure: Mood effects on the intimacy and reciprocity of disclosing personal information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (3): 449–461. doi:10.1037/a0021129. PMID 21171788.

Further reading

  • Beiser, M (1974). "Components and correlates of mental well-being". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 15 (4): 320–327. doi:10.2307/2137092. JSTOR 2137092. PMID 4455735.
  • Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.
  • DeNeve, K. M.; Cooper, H. (1998). "The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being". Psychological Bulletin. 124 (2): 197–229. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197. PMID 9747186.
  • Jex, S. M.; Spector, P. E. (1996). "The impact of negative affectivity on stressor strain relations: A replication and extension". Work and Stress. 10: 36–45. doi:10.1080/02678379608256783.
  • Judge, T. A.; Locke, E. A.; Durham, C. C.; Kluger, A. N. (1998). "Dispositional effects on job and life satisfaction: the role of core evaluations". Journal of Applied Psychology. 83 (1): 17–34. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.1.17. PMID 9494439.
  • Kanner, A. D.; Coyne, J. C.; Schaefer, C.; Lazarus, R. S. (1981). "). Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events". Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 4 (1): 1–39. doi:10.1007/bf00844845. PMID 7288876. S2CID 13229341.
  • Larsen, R. J.; Ketelaar, T. (1989). "Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures" (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (12): 1221–1228. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90233-x. hdl:2027.42/28239.
  • Norton, P. (2011). Risk Factors in the Development of Anxiety Disorders: Negative Affectivity. "Strides", 2–5.
  • Pressman, S. & Cohen, S. (2006). Positive Affect and Health. "Current Directions in Psychological Science, Carnegie Mellon University" 122–125.
  • Sheppard, L., Krammer, G., & Bodenhausen, G. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: the differential impact of anger and sadness. "European Journal of Social Psychology", 46–51.
  • Stone, A. A. (1981). "The association between perceptions of daily experiences and self- and spouse-rated mood". Journal of Research in Personality. 15 (4): 510–522. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(81)90047-7.

Read other articles:

مقدمة تعديل Flag of Madagascar Coat of Arms of Madagascar مدغشقر، أو جمهورية مدغشقر (الاسم القديم لها الجمهورية المالاجاشية)، هي دولة جزرية في المحيط الهندي قبالة الساحل الجنوبي الشرقي لأفريقيا. الجزيرة الرئيسية، اسمها أيضا مدغشقر، هي رابع أكبر جزيرة في العالم، ويعيش فيها 5% من الأنواع النباتي

Dieser Artikel behandelt die rheinland-pfälzische Ortsgemeinde Reiferscheid. Für den Ortsteil von Ruppichteroth in Nordrhein-Westfalen siehe Reiferscheid (Ruppichteroth). Zu Orten namens Reifferscheid siehe Gemeinde Reifferscheid im Landkreis Ahrweiler und Ortsteil Reifferscheid (Hellenthal) der Gemeinde Hellenthal. Wappen Deutschlandkarte ? Hilfe zu Wappen 50.6555555555567.5461111111111250Koordinaten: 50° 39′ N, 7° 33′ O Basisdaten Bundesland: Rheinland-Pfalz Land...

عنتأمراء فرنساالجيل الأول هم من أبناء هنري الرابع ملك فرنسا؛ رتب الذكور مثل ابن فرنسا أو حفيد فرنسا؛الجيل الأول لويس الثالث عشر ملك فرنسا نيكولاس هنري دوق أورليان* غاستون دوق أورليان* الجيل الثاني لويس الرابع عشر ملك فرنسا فيليب الأول دوق أورليان جان غاستون دوق فالوا* الجي...

Administrative division of the Ottoman Empire from 1827 to 1864 ایالت قسطمونیEyālet-i QasṭamōnīEyalet of the Ottoman Empire1827–1864The Kastamonu Eyalet in 1861CapitalKastamonu[1]HistoryHistory • Established 1827• Disestablished 1864 Preceded by Succeeded by Anatolia Eyalet Kastamonu Vilayet Today part ofTurkey Kastamonu Eyalet[1] (Ottoman Turkish: ایالت قسطمونی, romanized: Eyālet-i Qasṭamōnī)[2] was an eyalet...

Ця стаття містить перелік посилань, але походження тверджень у ній залишається незрозумілим через практично повну відсутність внутрішньотекстових джерел-виносок. Будь ласка, допоможіть поліпшити цю статтю, перетворивши джерела з переліку посилань на джерела-виноски у...

هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (أبريل 2019) ليز برنس   معلومات شخصية الميلاد 18 ديسمبر 1981 (42 سنة)  بوسطن  مواطنة الولايات المتحدة  الحياة العملية المهنة رسامة كارتون،  وفنانة قصص مصورة،  و�...

Worship of the self Mha PujāSketch of Nepalese Mha Pujā MandalaObserved byNepalese Hindus and BuddhistsTypeReligiousSignificanceSpiritual purification for new yearCelebrationsMandala worship, ritual gifts presentation, feastDateKachhala New Moon Mha Pujā (Newar Script:

British book and magazine publisher HutchinsonParent companyPenguin Random HouseFounded1887; 136 years ago (1887)FounderGeorge HutchinsonCountry of originUnited KingdomPublication typesBooksOfficial websitewww.penguin.co.uk/company/publishers/cornerstone/hutchinson.html Hutchinson was a British publishing firm which operated from 1887 until 1985, when it underwent several mergers. It is currently an imprint which is ultimately owned by Bertelsmann, the German publishing cong...

Human settlement in Butte County, California This article relies largely or entirely on a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.Find sources: Totoma, California – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (April 2021) Totoma (also, To-to and Totu) is a former Maidu settlement in Butte County, California. It was located on the east side of the n...

1994 studio album by YanoYanoStudio album by YanoReleasedJune 30, 1994 (re-released 1998)GenreAlternative rock, punk rockLabelAlpha Records&BMG Records (Pilipinas) Inc.ProducerRudy Y. TeeYano chronology Yano(1994) Bawal(1996) Singles from Yano Naroon[1]Released: 1995 Professional ratingsReview scoresSourceRatingAllmusic[2] Yano is the debut album of Filipino rock band, Yano. It has 12 tracks and released under Alpha Records in 1994.[3][4] and re-rel...

2013 science fiction film directed by Caradog W. James Not to be confused with other films with a similar title, see Machine (disambiguation). The MachineTheatrical release posterDirected byCaradog W. JamesWritten byCaradog W. JamesProduced byJohn Giwa-AmuStarring Caity Lotz Sam Hazeldine Toby Stephens Pooneh Hajimohammadi Denis Lawson CinematographyNicolai BrüelEdited byMatt Platts-MillsMusic byTom RaybouldProductioncompanyRed & Black FilmsDistributed byContent MediaRelease date 20 ...

Organization providing vaccination information Immunization Action CoalitionFormation1994[1]TypeNGO[1]Legal statusNon-profit[1]FocusDistribution of information about vaccines and the diseases they preventHeadquartersSaint Paul, MinnesotaRegion United StatesWebsiteimmunize.org The Immunization Action Coalition (IAC) is an organization that distributes information about vaccines and the diseases they prevent.[2] In partnership with the Centers for Disease Control...

Filipino actor, film director, and politician FPJ and Da King redirect here. For other uses, see Fernando Poe (disambiguation), FPJ (disambiguation), and The King (disambiguation). In this Philippine name, the middle name or maternal family name is Kelley and the surname or paternal family name is Poe. This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Fin...

Japanese urban legend about a legless spirit This article is about Japanese urban legend. For the 2009 film, see Teketeke (film). An artist's depiction of Teke Teke Teke Teke (テケテケ;てけ てけ),[1] also spelled Teke-Teke,[2] Teketeke,[3] or Teke teke,[1] is a Japanese urban legend about the ghost of a schoolgirl who is said to have fallen onto a railway line under the overpass, where her body was cut in half by a train. She is an onryō, or a vengeful...

ヌマーナNumana 行政国 イタリア州 マルケ県/大都市 アンコーナCAP(郵便番号) 60026市外局番 071ISTATコード 042032識別コード F978分離集落 #分離集落参照隣接コムーネ #隣接コムーネ参照気候分類 zona D, 1789 GG公式サイト リンク人口人口 3,767 [1] 人 (2017-01-01)人口密度 350.7 人/km2文化住民の呼称 numanesi守護聖人 Cristo Re祝祭日 5月3日地理座標 北緯43度30分48.96秒 東経13度37分17...

Makarov PMПистолет Макарова ПМ Makarov PM Jenis Pistol Semi Otomatis Negara asal Uni Sovyet Sejarah pemakaian Digunakan oleh Uni Sovyet, Albania, Bulgaria, Estonia, Jerman Timur, Kamboja, Latvia, Republik Rakyat Tiongkok, Vietnam Indonesia Pada perang Perang Soviet-Afganistan, Perang Vietnam Sejarah produksi Perancang Nikolai Fyodorovich Makarov Produsen Izhevsk, RusiaVEB Fahrzeug- und Jagdwaffenwerk Ernst Thälmann Suhl, Jerman TimurNorinco, Republik Rakyat T...

Ada usul agar Dawet ayu rumput laut digabungkan ke artikel ini. (Diskusikan) Ada usul agar Dawet ayu digabungkan ke artikel ini. (Diskusikan) Ada usul agar Dawet ireng digabungkan ke artikel ini. (Diskusikan) DawetJenisMinumanTempat asalJabung, IndonesiaDaerahPulau JawaMasakan nasional terkaitIndonesiaSuhu penyajianDingin/suhu ruangVariasiDawet ayu Dawet ayu rumput laut Dawet ireng Dawet Semarang Dawet Jepara Dawet Jabung Dawet (Jawa: ꦝꦮꦼꦠ꧀, translit. dhawêt) adalah m...

1989 Hong Kong filmDragon FightTraditional Chinese龍在天涯Simplified Chinese龙在天涯Hanyu PinyinLóng zài tiān yáJyutpingLung4 zoi6 tin1 ngaai4 Directed byBilly TangWritten byJames YuenProduced byHenry FongStarringJet LiNina Li ChiStephen ChowDick WeiCinematographyLouis YuenChu Chi-hungRumjamn Abdul MohammedEdited bySiu LamMusic byViolet LamDistributed byGrand March Movie ProductionRelease date 1 September 1989 (1989-09-01) Running time96 minutesCountryHong Kong...

此條目需要擴充。 (2011年9月14日)请協助改善这篇條目,更進一步的信息可能會在討論頁或扩充请求中找到。请在擴充條目後將此模板移除。 花生四烯酸 IUPAC名(5Z,8Z,11Z,14Z)-5,8,11,14-Eicosatetraenoic acid 系统名(5Z,8Z,11Z,14Z)-Icosa-5,8,11,14-tetraenoic acid[1] 英文名 Arachidonic acid 别名 全順式-5,8,11,14-二十碳四烯酸 all-cis-5,8,11,14-Eicosatetraenoic acid 缩写 AA; ARA 识别 CAS号 506-32-1  Y PubChem 44489...

ARA Heroína (D-12) ARA Heroína.Banderas HistorialAstillero Blohm + VossClase MEKO 360Tipo DestructorOperador Armada ArgentinaAutorizado 29 de enero de 1979Botado 17 de febrero de 1982Asignado 7 de noviembre de 1983 (afirmación del Pabellón Argentino)21 de diciembre de 1983 (incorporación a la Flota de Mar)Destino En servicio en la División de Destructores de la Flota de Mar, Base Naval Puerto BelgranoCaracterísticas generalesDesplazamiento 3600 t a plena cargaEslora 125 mMang...