Intersubjectivity

In philosophy, psychology, sociology, and anthropology, intersubjectivity is the relation or intersection between people's cognitive perspectives.

Definition

Intersubjectivity is a term coined by social scientists beginning around 1970 to refer to a variety of types of human interaction. The term was introduced to psychoanalysis by George E. Atwood and Robert Stolorow, who consider it a "meta-theory" of psychoanalysis.[1] For example, social psychologists Alex Gillespie and Flora Cornish listed at least seven definitions of intersubjectivity (and other disciplines have additional definitions):

  • people's agreement on the shared definition of a concept;
  • people's mutual awareness of agreement or disagreement, or of understanding or misunderstanding each other;
  • people's attribution of intentionality, feelings, and beliefs to each other;
  • people's implicit or automatic behavioral orientations towards other people;
  • people's interactive performance within a situation;
  • people's shared and taken-for-granted background assumptions, whether consensual or contested; and
  • "the variety of possible relations between people's perspectives".[2]

Intersubjectivity has been used in social science to refer to agreement. There is intersubjectivity between people if they agree on a given set of meanings or share the same perception of a situation. Similarly, Thomas Scheff defines intersubjectivity as "the sharing of subjective states by two or more individuals".[3]

Intersubjectivity also has been used to refer to the common-sense, shared meanings constructed by people in their interactions with each other and used as an everyday resource to interpret the meaning of elements of social and cultural life. If people share common sense, then they share a definition of the situation.[4]

Psychoanalyst Jessica Benjamin, in The Bonds of Love, wrote, "The concept of intersubjectivity has its origins in the social theory of Jürgen Habermas (1970), who used the expression 'the intersubjectivity of mutual understanding' to designate an individual capacity and a social domain."[5]: 19  Psychoanalyst Molly Macdonald argued in 2011 that a "potential point of origin" for the term was in Jean Hyppolite's use of l'inter-subjectivité in an essay from 1955 on "The Human Situation in the Hegelian Phenomenology".[6] However, the phenomenologist Edmund Husserl, whose work Habermas and Hyppolite draw upon, was the first to develop the term, which was subsequently elaborated upon by other phenomenologists such as Edith Stein, Emmanuel Levinas, and Maurice Merleau-Ponty.

Philosophy

Contemporarily, intersubjectivity is the major topic in both the analytic and the continental traditions of philosophy. Intersubjectivity is considered crucial not only at the relational level but also at the epistemological and even metaphysical levels. For example, intersubjectivity is postulated as playing a role in establishing the truth of propositions, and constituting the intersubjective agreement of an experience of an object.

A central concern in consciousness studies of the past 50 years is the so-called problem of other minds, which asks how we can justify our belief that people have minds much like our own and predict others' mind-states and behavior, as our experience shows we often can.[7] Contemporary philosophical theories of intersubjectivity need to address the problem of other minds.

In the debate between cognitive individualism and cognitive universalism, some aspects of thinking are neither solely personal nor fully universal. Cognitive sociology proponents argue for intersubjectivity—an intermediate perspective of social cognition that provides a balanced view between personal and universal views of our social cognition. This approach suggests that, instead of being individual or universal thinkers, human beings subscribe to "thought communities"—communities of differing beliefs. Thought community examples include churches, professions, scientific beliefs, generations, nations, and political movements.[8] This perspective explains why each individual thinks differently from another (individualism): person A may choose to adhere to expiry dates on foods, but person B may believe that expiry dates are only guidelines and it is still safe to eat the food days past the expiry date. But not all human beings think the same way (universalism).

Intersubjectivity argues that each thought community shares social experiences that are different from the social experiences of other thought communities, creating differing beliefs among people who subscribe to different thought communities. These experiences transcend our subjectivity, which explains why they can be shared by the entire thought community.[8] Proponents of intersubjectivity support the view that individual beliefs are often the result of thought community beliefs, not just personal experiences or universal and objective human beliefs. Beliefs are recast in terms of standards, which are set by thought communities.

Phenomenology

Edmund Husserl, the founder of phenomenology, recognized the importance of intersubjectivity, and wrote extensively on the topic. In German, his writings on intersubjectivity are gathered in volumes 13–15 of the Husserliana. In English, his best-known text on intersubjectivity is the Cartesian Meditations (it is this text that features solely in the Husserl reader entitled The Essential Husserl). Although Husserlian phenomenology is often charged with methodological solipsism, in the fifth Cartesian Meditation, Husserl attempts to grapple with the problem of intersubjectivity and puts forward his theory of transcendental, monadological intersubjectivity.[9]

Husserl's student Edith Stein extended intersubjectivity's basis in empathy in her 1917 doctoral dissertation, On the Problem of Empathy (Zum Problem der Einfühlung).

Psychology

Discussions and theories of intersubjectivity are prominent and of importance in contemporary psychology, theory of mind, and consciousness studies. Three major contemporary theories of intersubjectivity are theory theory, simulation theory, and interaction theory.

Shannon Spaulding, Assistant Professor of Philosophy at Oklahoma State University, wrote:

Theory theorists argue that we explain and predict behaviour by employing folk psychological theories about how mental states inform behaviour. With our folk psychological theories, we infer from a target's behaviour what his or her mental states probably are. And from these inferences, plus the psychological principles in the theory connecting mental states to behavior, we predict the target's behaviour (Carruthers and Smith 1996; Davies and Stone 1995a; Gopnik and Wellman 1992; Nichols and Stich 2003).[10]

Simulation theorists, on the other hand, claim that we explain and predict others' behaviour by using our own minds as a model and "putting ourselves in another's shoes"—that is, by imagining what our mental states would be and how we would behave if we were in the other's situation. More specifically, we simulate what the other's mental states could have been to cause the observed behaviour, then use the simulated mental states, pretend beliefs, and pretend desires as input, running them through our own decision-making mechanism. We then take the resulting conclusion and attribute it to the other person.[10] Authors like Vittorio Gallese have proposed a theory of embodied simulation that refers to neuroscientific research on mirror neurons and phenomenological research.[11]

Spaulding noted that this debate has stalled in the past few years, with progress limited to articulating various hybrid simulation theories—"theory theory" accounts.[10] To resolve this impasse, authors like Shaun Gallagher put forward interaction theory. Gallagher writes that an "... important shift is taking place in social cognition research, away from a focus on the individual mind and toward ... participatory aspects of social understanding...." Interaction theory is put forward to "galvanize" the interactive turn in explanations of intersubjectivity.[12] Gallagher defines an interaction as two or more autonomous agents engaged in co-regulated coupling behavior. For example, when walking a dog, both the owner's behavior is regulated by the dog stopping and sniffing, and the dog's behavior is regulated by the lead and the owner's commands. Ergo, walking the dog is an example of an interactive process. For Gallagher, interaction and direct perception constitute what he terms "primary" (or basic) intersubjectivity.

Studies of dialogue and dialogism reveal how language is deeply intersubjective. When we speak, we always address our interlocutors, taking their perspective and orienting to what we think they think (or, more often, don't think).[13] Within this tradition of research, it has been argued that the structure of individual signs or symbols, the basis of language, is intersubjective[14] and that the psychological process of self-reflection entails intersubjectivity.[15] Recent research on mirror neurons provides evidence for the deeply intersubjective basis of human psychology,[16] and arguably much of the literature on empathy and theory of mind relates directly to intersubjectivity.

In child development

Colwyn Trevarthen has applied intersubjectivity to the very rapid cultural development of new born infants.[17] Research suggests that as babies, humans are biologically wired to "coordinate their actions with others".[18] This ability to coordinate and sync with others facilitates cognitive and emotional learning through social interaction. Additionally, the most socially productive relationship between children and adults is bidirectional, where both parties actively define a shared culture.[18] The bidirectional aspect lets the active parties organize the relationship how they see fit—what they see as important receives the most focus. Emphasis is placed on the idea that children are actively involved in how they learn, using intersubjectivity.[18]

Across cultures

The ways intersubjectivity occurs varies across cultures. In certain Indigenous American communities, nonverbal communication is so prevalent that intersubjectivity may occur regularly amongst all members of the community, in part perhaps due to a "joint cultural understanding" and a history of shared endeavors.[19] This "joint cultural understanding" may develop in small, Indigenous American communities where children have grown up embedded in their community's values, expectations, and livelihoods—learning through participation with adults rather than through intent verbal instruction—working in cohesion with one another in shared endeavors on a daily basis. Having grown up within this context may have led to members of this community to have what is described by some as a "blending of agendas",[19] or by others as a "dovetailing of motives".[20] If community or family members have the same general goals in mind they may thus act cohesively within an overlapping state of mind. Whether persons are in each other's presence or merely within the same community this blending of agendas or dovetailing of motives enables intersubjectivity to occur within these shared endeavors.[19]

The cultural value of respeto may also contribute to intersubjectivity in some communities; unlike the English definition of 'respect', respeto refers loosely to a mutual consideration for others' activities, needs, wants, etc.[19] Similar to "putting yourself in another's shoes" the prevalence of respeto in certain Indigenous American communities in Mexico and South America may promote intersubjectivity as persons act in accordance with one another within consideration for the community or the individual's current needs or state of mind.

Shared reference during an activity facilitates learning. Adults either teach by doing the task with children, or by directing attention toward experts. Children that had to ask questions in regard to how to perform a task were scolded for not learning by another's example, as though they were ignoring the available resources to learn a task, as seen in Tz'utujil Maya parents who scolded questioning children and asking "if they had eyes".[21]

Children from the Chillihuani village in the Andean mountains learned to weave without explicit instruction. They learned the basic technique from others by observing, eager to participate in their community. The learning process was facilitated by watching adults and by being allowed to play and experiment using tools to create their own weaving techniques.[22]

See also

References

  1. ^ Spezzano, C. (2012). Textbook of psychoanalysis (2nd ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing. pp. 105–116. ISBN 978-1-58562-410-2.
  2. ^ Gillespie, Alex; Cornish, Flora (March 2010). "Intersubjectivity: towards a dialogical analysis" (PDF). Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour. 40 (1): 19–46. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.724.7095. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5914.2009.00419.x. hdl:1893/2576.
  3. ^ Scheff, Thomas J. (2015-11-15). Goffman Unbound!: A New Paradigm for Social Science. New York: Routledge. ISBN 978-1-315-63435-7.
  4. ^ Seale, Clive (1998). Researching society and culture. London: Sage Publications. ISBN 978-0-7619-5276-3.
  5. ^ Benjamin, Jessica (July 12, 1988). The Bonds of Love: Psychoanalysis, Feminism, & the Problem of Domination. Pantheon. ISBN 0394757300.
  6. ^ Macdonald, M (2011) "Hegel, Psychoanalysis and Intersubjectivity" in Philosophy Compass, 6/7 p449
  7. ^ Hyslop, A (2010). "Other Minds", The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall Edition), Edward N. Zalta (Ed.) Accessed from plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2010/entries/other-minds/>. Section 1.
  8. ^ a b Zerubavel, Eviatar (1997). Social Mindscapes: An Invitation to Cognitive Sociology. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
  9. ^ E. Husserl, Cartesian Meditations, Klumer Academic Publishers. Translated by Dorion Cairns.
  10. ^ a b c Spaulding, Shannon (2012-09-05). "Introduction to debates on Social Cognition". Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. 11 (4): 431–448 (432, 433). doi:10.1007/s11097-012-9275-x. ISSN 1568-7759.
  11. ^ Gallese, V & Sinigaglia, C. (2011) What is so special about embodied simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. Vol. 15, No. 11.
  12. ^ De Jaeger, H., Di Paulo, E., & Gallagher, S. (2010) Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. Vol. 14, No. 10. Pg 441. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009
  13. ^ Linell, P. (2009). Rethinking language, mind and world dialogically. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing
  14. ^ Gillespie, A. (2009). The intersubjective nature of symbols. In Brady Wagoner (Ed), Symbolic transformations. London: Routledge
  15. ^ Gillespie, A. (2007). The social basis of self-reflection. In Valsiner and Rosa (Eds), The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University press
  16. ^ Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in neurosciences, 21, 188-194.
  17. ^ Trevarthen, Colwyn (January 2011). "What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being". Infant and Child Development. 20 (1): 119–135. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.475.9911. doi:10.1002/icd.689.
  18. ^ a b c Stone, Lynda; Underwood, Charles; Hotchkiss, Jacqueline (2012). "The Relational Habitus: Intersubjective Processes in Learning Settings". Human Development. 55 (2): 65–91. doi:10.1159/000337150. S2CID 144722047. Retrieved 10 December 2014.
  19. ^ a b c d Correa-Chávez, M., & Roberts, A. (2012). A cultural analysis is necessary in understanding intersubjectivity. Culture & Psychology, 18(1), 99-108. doi: 10.1177/1354067X11427471
  20. ^ Danziger, E., & Rumsey, A. (2013). Introduction: From Opacity to intersubjectivity across languages and cultures. Language & Communication, 33(3), 247-250.
  21. ^ Paradise, Ruth; Rogoff, Barbara (2009). "Side by Side: Learning by Observing and Pitching In". Ethos. 37 (1): 102–138. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1352.2009.01033.x.
  22. ^ Bolin, Inge (2006). Growing Up in a Culture of Respect: Childrearing in highland Peru (2 ed.). Austin: University of Texas. pp. 90–99. ISBN 978-0-292-71298-0.

Further reading

Psychoanalysis

  • Brandchaft, Doctors & Sorter (2010). Toward an Emancipatory Psychoanalysis. Routledge: New York.
  • Laplanche, J. & Pontalis, J. B. (1974). The Language of Psycho-Analysis, Edited by W. W. Norton & Company, ISBN 0-393-01105-4
  • Orange, Atwood & Stolorow (1997). Working Intersubjectively. The Analytic Press: Hillsdale, NJ.
  • Stolorow, R. D., Atwood, G. E., & Orange, D. M. (2002). Worlds of Experience: Interweaving Philosophical and Clinical Dimensions in Psychoanalysis. New York: Basic Books.
  • Stolorow & Atwood (1992). Contexts of Being. The Analytic Press: Hillsdale, NJ.
  • Stolorow, Brandchaft & Atwood (1987). Psychoanalytic Treatment: An Intersubjective Approach. The Analytic Press:Hillsdale, NJ.

Philosophy

Read other articles:

Campeonato Mundial Júnior de Patinação Artística no Gelo de 1981 Dados Tipo Campeonato ISU Data 8 de dezembro–14 de dezembro de 1980 Temporada 1980–1981 Cidade London, Ontário, Canadá Campeões Individual masculino Paul Wylie Individual feminino Tiffany Chin Duplas Larisa Selezneva / Oleg Makarov Dança no gelo Elena Batanova / Alexei Soloviev Cronologia da competição Megève 1980 Oberstdorf 1982 O Campeonato Mundial Júnior de Patinação Artística no Gelo de 1981 foi a sexta ed...

Координати: 47°57′02″ пн. ш. 33°26′56″ сх. д. / 47.9505° пн. ш. 33.4489° сх. д. / 47.9505; 33.4489 Міська лікарня Криворізький метрополітен Вхід до станції ПлатформаЗагальні даніТип наземна, відкритаПроєктна назва СхіднаТип 2Форма берегові, на дузіДовжина 45 м�...

?†AivukusЧас існування: Міоцен (7,246—5,332 млн років тому) Реконструкція голови Aivukus cedrosensis sp. Біологічна класифікація Домен: Ядерні (Eukaryota) Царство: Тварини (Animalia) Тип: Хордові (Chordata) Клас: Ссавці (Mammalia) Ряд: Хижі (Carnivora) Підряд: Псовиді (Caniformia) Надродина: Ластоногі (Pinnipedia) Родина: Мор

American TV series or program Wonder WomanGenre Action Science fantasy Superhero Based onWonder Womanby William Moulton MarstonH. G. PeterWritten byDavid E. KelleyDirected byJeffrey ReinerStarring Adrianne Palicki Cary Elwes Pedro Pascal Elizabeth Hurley Edward Herrmann Tracie Thoms Justin Bruening Theme music composerChris BaconOpening themeI Only Know How to Love by Christina AguileraCountry of originUnited StatesOriginal languageEnglishProductionExecutive producersBill D'EliaDavid E. ...

El morcón un embutido rojo del pimentón, una característica de los embutidos extremeños de la provincia. Sopa de Antruejo. La patatera. Otro embutido típico de la gastromía extremeña La gastronomía de la provincia de Badajoz es el conjunto de platos y costumbres culinarias de la provincia de Badajoz (Extremadura).[1]​ Se trata de la provincia más al sur de Extremadura y ejerce influencia en la cocina de Huelva. Uno de los productos estrella de la provincia es la abundante chaci...

2010 film by Jean-Luc Godard Film SocialismeEarly promotional one-sheet posterDirected byJean-Luc GodardProduced byAlain SardeCinematographyFabrice AragnoPaul GrivasProductioncompanyVega FilmDistributed byWild BunchRelease dates 17 May 2010 (2010-05-17) (Cannes) 19 May 2010 (2010-05-19) (France) Running time102 minutesCountryFranceLanguageFrenchBox office$410,000[1] Film Socialisme (alternative French title Socialisme; English: Socialism but often...

Wajra sebagai alat ritual Viśvavajra atau wajra ganda ditampilkan di lambang Bhutan. Wajra (Dewanagari: वज्र; ,IAST: Vajra,; Jawa: Wojro; Bali: Bajra) adalah istilah yang berasal dari bahasa Sanskerta yang bemakna halilintar dan intan.[1] Sebagai benda ritual, wajra adalah tongkat logam dengan sula ditengahnya dikelilingi banyak sula-sula lain yang melengkung ke dalam dan ujungnya menyambung bersatu dengan sula utama di tengahnya. Alat ritual Wajra dipercaya me...

سلوقية (سفينة)معلومات عامةصنف فرعي من عنصر من سفينة تعديل - تعديل مصدري - تعديل ويكي بيانات نسخة طبق الأصل من فِكتورية، وهي السفينة الوحيدة من بين سفن ماجِلّان الخمس التي عادت إلى إسبانيا في عام 1522 والتي تُظهر كلاً من السلوقية عن يسار. السَّلوقية[1] هو السطح الأعلى لسفينة...

Đối với các định nghĩa khác, xem Kim Tiến. Thầy thuốc nhân dânNguyễn Thị Kim TiếnNguyễn Thị Kim Tiếntháng 12 năm 2020 tại Hà NộiChức vụTrưởng ban Bảo vệ, Chăm sóc sức khỏe cán bộ trung ươngNhiệm kỳ5 tháng 7 năm 2019 – 19 tháng 11 năm 20212 năm, 137 ngàyTiền nhiệmNguyễn Quốc TriệuKế nhiệmNguyễn Thanh Long Bộ trưởng Bộ Y tế Việt NamNhiệm kỳ3 tháng 8 năm 2011 R...

Félix Hernández GiménezInformación personalNacimiento 21 de junio de 1889 Barcelona (España) Fallecimiento 17 de mayo de 1975 Córdoba (España) Nacionalidad EspañolaInformación profesionalOcupación Arquitecto Distinciones Gran Cruz de la Orden Civil de Alfonso X el Sabio (1966) [editar datos en Wikidata] Félix Hernández Giménez (n. Barcelona; 21 de junio de 1889 - f. Córdoba; 17 de mayo de 1975) fue un arquitecto español. Biografía Nació en 1889 en Barcelona, ad...

Shri Rawatpura Sarkar UniversityEstablished2018; 5 years ago (2018)ChancellorRavi Shankar Ji Maharaj[1]Vice-ChancellorS.K. Singh[1]LocationDhaneli, Raipur, Chhattisgarh, IndiaAffiliationsUGCWebsitesruraipur.ac.in Shri Rawatpura Sarkar University (SRU) is a private university[2] located at the village Dhaneli in Raipur district, Chhattisgarh, India. It was established in 2018 by the Shri Rawatpura Sarkar Lok Kalyan Trust under the Chhattisgarh Private ...

ДостопримечательностьЦентр культуры и искусства на Соборной 56°07′46″ с. ш. 40°24′26″ в. д.HGЯO Страна  Россия Владимир Большая Московская улица, 33/35 Архитектурный стиль русский классицизм Автор проекта Василий Дрегалов,Ефграф Петров Дата основания 1826 год Стату�...

In this Japanese name, the surname is Kumagai. Kumagai is often depicted riding his horse backwards. Though likely more myth than historical fact, he is said to have been determined to always face towards the Western Paradise of Amida, even when riding to the east, as part of his penance and devotion.[1] Kumagai no Jirō Naozane (熊谷 次郎 直実) (March 24, 1141 – September 27, 1207/October 25, 1208) was a famous soldier who served the Genji (Minamoto) clan during the Heian per...

Seljuk prince (died 1073) QavurtGold dinar under Qavurt, citing Chaghri Beg as his overlord. Minted in Jiroft, dated 1056/7Born1040sDied1073NamesKara Arslan Ahmad Qavurt bin Dawud ChaghriHouseSeljukFatherDawud Chaghri BegReligionSunni IslamMilitary careerYears of servicec. 1060s – 1073Battles/warsSeljuq War of succession Kara Arslan Ahmad Qavurt (died 1073), better simply known as Qavurt (also spelled Kavurt) was a Seljuq prince. Upon his brother's death, he led an unsuccessful rebelli...

Class I railroad in Canada and the United States For the successor company, see Canadian Pacific Kansas City. Canadian Pacific redirects here. For other uses, see Canadian Pacific (disambiguation).It has been suggested that this article should be split into a new article titled History of the Canadian Pacific Railway. (discuss) (December 2023)This article may be too long to read and navigate comfortably. Please consider splitting content into sub-articles, condensing it, or adding subheadings...

2008 Lebanese filmMelodrama HabibiFrench posterDirected byHany TambaWritten byHany TambaProduced byEmmanuel Agneray Jérôme BleitrachStarringJulia Kassar Patrick Chesnais Pierre Chammassian Joanna Moukarzel Pierrette Katrib Gabriel Yammine Lara Mattar Maggie BadawiCinematographyEmmanuel SoyerMusic byKhaled MouzanarDistributed byHaut et Court Films DistributionsRelease date 13 August 2008 (2008-08-13) Running time98 minutesCountryLebanonLanguagesArabic and French Melodrama Habi...

Not to be confused with standard atomic weight, mass number, relative atomic mass, atomic mass unit, or atomic number. For the musical group, see Atomic Mass (band). Rest mass of an atom in its ground state This article needs to be updated. The reason given is: it needs to reflect the 2019 redefinition of the SI base units, which came into effect on 20 May 2019. Please help update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information. (January 2020) Atomic massStylized lithium-...

Office building in Halifax, Nova Scotia Maritime CentreNortheastern facadeGeneral informationStatusCompletedTypeOffice, retailArchitectural styleModernismLocation1505 Barrington StreetHalifax, Nova ScotiaConstruction started1974Opened1977ClientMT&TOwnerSlate Office REITDesign and constructionArchitect(s)Webb Zerafa Menkes HousdenDeveloperTrizec EquitiesStructural engineerGeorge Brandys & Associates Ltd.Main contractorRobert McAlpine Ltd.Websitemaritimecentre.ca 44°38′40″N 63°34�...

Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: SMA Negeri 1 Sangatta – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR Sekolah Menengah Atas Negeri 1 Sangatta Data Sekolah Berdiri: 24 Juni 1997 Provinsi: Kalimantan Timur Alamat Lengkap: Jalan A. Wa...

For the Haltdalen church built in 1881, see Haltdalen Church. Haltdalen Stave Church at Sverresborg Trøndelag Folkemuseum Haltdalen Stave Church (Norwegian: Haltdalen stavkyrkje) is a stave church that was originally built in the 1170s in the village of Haltdalen in what is now the municipality of Holtålen in Trøndelag county, Norway. The church is now on display at the Sverresborg Trøndelag Folkemuseum in the city of Trondheim. This is a single-nave stave church of the east Scandinavian-...