Naïve realism (psychology)

In social psychology, naïve realism is the human tendency to believe that we see the world around us objectively, and that people who disagree with us must be uninformed, irrational, or biased.

Naïve realism provides a theoretical basis for several other cognitive biases, which are systematic errors when it comes to thinking and making decisions. These include the false consensus effect, actor–observer bias, bias blind spot, and fundamental attribution error, among others.

The term, as it is used in psychology today, was coined by social psychologist Lee Ross and his colleagues in the 1990s.[1][2] It is related to the philosophical concept of naïve realism, which is the idea that our senses allow us to perceive objects directly and without any intervening processes.[3] Social psychologists in the mid-20th century argued against this stance and proposed instead that perception is inherently subjective.[4]

Several prominent social psychologists have studied naïve realism experimentally, including Lee Ross, Andrew Ward, Dale Griffin, Emily Pronin, Thomas Gilovich, Robert Robinson, and Dacher Keltner. In 2010, the Handbook of Social Psychology recognized naïve realism as one of "four hard-won insights about human perception, thinking, motivation and behavior that ... represent important, indeed foundational, contributions of social psychology."[5]

Main assumptions

Lee Ross and fellow psychologist Andrew Ward have outlined three interrelated assumptions, or "tenets", that make up naïve realism. They argue that these assumptions are supported by a long line of thinking in social psychology, along with several empirical studies. According to their model, people:

  • Believe that they see the world objectively and without bias.
  • Expect that others will come to the same conclusions, so long as they are exposed to the same information and interpret it in a rational manner.
  • Assume that others who do not share the same views must be ignorant, irrational, or biased.[1]

History of the concept

Naïve realism follows from a subjectivist tradition in modern social psychology, which traces its roots back to one of the field's founders, German-American psychologist Kurt Lewin.[1][6] Lewin's ideas were strongly informed by Gestalt psychology, a 20th-century school of thought which focused on examining psychological phenomena in context, as parts of a whole.[7]

From the 1920s through the 1940s, Lewin developed an approach for studying human behavior which he called field theory.[8] Field theory proposes that a person's behavior is a function of the person and the environment.[9] Lewin considered a person's psychological environment, or "life space", to be subjective and thus distinct from physical reality.[4]

During this time period, subjectivist ideas also propagated throughout other areas of psychology. For example, the developmental psychologist Jean Piaget argued that children view the world through an egocentric lens, and they have trouble separating their own beliefs from the beliefs of others.[10]

In the 1940s and 1950s, early pioneers in social psychology applied the subjectivist view to the field of social perception. In 1948, psychologists David Kretch and Richard Krutchfield argued that people perceive and interpret the world according to their "own needs, own connotations, own personality, own previously formed cognitive patterns".[1][11][12]

Social psychologist Gustav Ichheiser expanded on this idea, noting how biases in person perception lead to misunderstandings in social relations. According to Ichheiser, "We tend to resolve our perplexity arising out of the experience that other people see the world differently than we see it ourselves by declaring that these others, in consequence of some basic intellectual and moral defect, are unable to see things 'as they really are' and to react to them 'in a normal way'. We thus imply, of course, that things are in fact as we see them, and that our ways are the normal ways."[13]

Solomon Asch, a prominent social psychologist who was also brought up in the Gestalt tradition, argued that people disagree because they base their judgments on different construals, or ways of looking at various issues.[6][14] However, they are under the illusion that their judgments about the social world are objective. "This attitude, which has been aptly described as naive realism, sees no problem in the fact of perception or knowledge of the surroundings. Things are what they appear to be; they have just the qualities that they reveal to sight and touch," he wrote in his textbook Social Psychology in 1952. "This attitude, does not, however, describe the actual conditions of our knowledge of the surroundings."[15]

Experimental evidence

"They saw a game"

In a seminal study in social psychology, which was published in a paper in 1954, students from Dartmouth and Princeton watched a video of a heated football game between the two schools.[16] Though they looked at the same footage, fans from both schools perceived the game very differently. The Princeton students "saw" the Dartmouth team make twice as many infractions as their own team, and they also saw the team make twice as many infractions compared to what the Dartmouth students saw. Dartmouth students viewed the game as being evenly-matched in violence, in which both sides were to blame. This study revealed that two groups perceived an event subjectively. Each team believed they saw the event objectively and that the other side's perception of the event was blinded by bias.[1]

False consensus effect

A 1977 study conducted by Ross and colleagues provided early evidence for a cognitive bias called the false consensus effect, which is the tendency for people to overestimate the extent to which others share the same views.[17] This bias has been cited as supporting the first two tenets of naïve realism.[1][5] In the study, students were asked whether they would wear a sandwich-board sign, which said "Eat At Joe's" on it, around campus. Then they were asked to indicate whether they thought other students were likely to wear the sign, and what they thought about students who were either willing to wear it or not. The researchers found that students who agreed to wear the sign thought that the majority of students would wear the sign, and they thought that refusing to wear the sign was more revealing of their peers' personal attributes. Conversely, students who declined to wear the sign thought that most other students would also refuse, and that accepting the invitation was more revealing of certain personality traits.

Hostile media effect

A phenomenon referred to as the hostile media effect demonstrates that partisans can view neutral events subjectively according to their own needs and values, and make the assumption that those who interpret the event differently are biased. For a study in 1985, pro-Israeli and pro-Arab students were asked to watch real news coverage on the 1982 Sabra and Shatila massacre, a massive killing of Palestinian refugees (Vallone, Lee Ross and Lepper, 1985).[5][18] Researchers found that partisans from both sides perceived the coverage as being biased in favor of the opposite viewpoint, and believed that the people in charge of the news program held the ideological views of the opposite side.

"Musical tapping" study

More empirical evidence for naïve realism came from psychologist Elizabeth Newton's "musical tapping study" in 1990. For the study, participants were designated either as "tappers" or as "listeners". The tappers were told to tap out the rhythm of a well-known song, while the "listeners" were asked to try to identify the song. While tappers expected that listeners would guess the tune around 50 percent of the time, the listeners were able to identify it only around 2.5 percent of the time. This provided support for a failure in perspective-taking on the side of the tappers, and an overestimation of the extent to which others would share in "hearing" the song as it was tapped.[1]

Wall Street Game

In 2004, Ross, Liberman, and Samuels asked dorm resident advisors to nominate students to participate in a study, and to indicate whether those students were likely to cooperate or defect in the first round of the classic decision-making game called the Prisoner's Dilemma. The game was introduced to subjects in one of two ways: it was either referred to as the "Wall Street Game" or as the "Community Game". The researchers found that students in the "Community Game" condition were twice as likely to cooperate, and that it did not seem to make a difference whether students were previously categorized as "cooperators" versus "defectors". This experiment demonstrated that the game's label exerted more power on how the students played the game than the subjects' personality traits. Furthermore, the study showed that the dorm advisors did not make sufficient allowances for subjective interpretations of the game.[1]

Consequences

Naïve realism causes people to exaggerate differences between themselves and others. Psychologists believe that it can spark and exacerbate conflict, as well as create barriers to negotiation through several different mechanisms.[11]

Bias blind spot

One consequence of naïve realism is referred to as the bias blind spot, which is the ability to recognize cognitive and motivational biases in others while failing to recognize the impact of bias on the self. In a study conducted by Pronin, Lin, and Ross (2002), Stanford students completed a questionnaire about various biases in social judgment.[19] The participants indicated how susceptible they thought they were to these biases compared to the average student. The researchers found that the participants consistently believed that they were less likely to be biased than their peers. In a follow-up study, students answered questions about their personal attributes (e.g. how considerate they were) compared to those of other students. The majority of students saw themselves as falling above average on most traits, which provided support for a cognitive bias known as the better-than-average effect. The students then were told that 70 to 80 percent of people fall prey to this bias. When asked about the accuracy of their self-assessments, 63 percent of the students argued that their ratings had been objective, while 13 percent of students indicated they thought their ratings had been too modest.[20]

Fig. 1. Actual views (top), "circle's" perception of views (middle), "triangle's" perception of views (bottom). (Modeled after similar illustrations found in Robinson et al., 1995, and Ross & Ward, 1996.)

False polarization

When an individual does not share our views, the third tenet of naïve realism attributes this discrepancy to three possibilities. The individual either has been exposed to a different set of information, is lazy or unable to come to a rational conclusion, or is under a distorting influence such as bias or self-interest.[1] This gives rise to a phenomenon called false polarization, which involves interpreting others' views as more extreme than they really are, and leads to a perception of greater intergroup differences (see Fig. 1).[6] People assume that they perceive the issue objectively, carefully considering it from multiple views, while the other side processes information in top-down fashion.[21] For instance, in a study conducted by Robinson et al. in 1996, pro-life and pro-choice partisans greatly overestimated the extremity of the views of the opposite side, and also overestimated the influence of ideology on others in their own group.[22]

Reactive devaluation

The assumption that others' views are more extreme than they are, can create a barrier for conflict resolution. In a sidewalk survey conducted in the 1980s, pedestrians evaluated a nuclear arms' disarmament proposal (Stillinger et al., 1991).[23] One group of participants was told that the proposal was made by American President Ronald Reagan, while others thought the proposal came from Soviet leader Mikhail Gorbachev. The researchers found that 90 percent of the participants who thought the proposal was from Reagan supported it, while only 44 percent in the Gorbachev group indicated their support. This provided support for a phenomenon called reactive devaluation, which involves dismissing a concession from an adversary on the assumption that the concession is either motivated by self-interest or less valuable.[11]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In T. Brown, E. S. Reed & E. Turiel (Eds.), Values and Knowledge (pp. 103–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  2. ^ Griffin, D., & Ross, L. (1991). Subjective construal, social inference, and human misunderstanding. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 319–359). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60333-0
  3. ^ Nuttall, John (2002). An Introduction to Philosophy. Maiden, MA: Wiley. ISBN 978-0-7456-1663-6.
  4. ^ a b Hergenhahn, B. (2008). An Introduction to the History of Psychology. Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-0-495-50621-8.
  5. ^ a b c Ross, L.; Lepper, M.; Ward, A., History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application. In Fiske, S. T., In Gilbert, D. T., In Lindzey, G., & Jongsma, A. E. (2010). Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol.1. Hoboken, N.J: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9780470561119.socpsy001001
  6. ^ a b c Robinson, Robert J.; Keltner, Dacher; Ward, Andrew; Ross, Lee (1995). "Actual versus assumed differences in construal: 'Naive realism' in intergroup perception and conflict". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 68 (3): 404–417. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.404. S2CID 16246373.
  7. ^ Gestalt psychology. (2015). In Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved from http://www.britannica.com/science/Gestalt-psychology
  8. ^ ""Field Theory". International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences. 1968. Retrieved November 17, 2015 from Encyclopedia.com
  9. ^ Lewin, Kurt (1939). "Field Theory and Experiment in Social Psychology: Concepts and Methods". American Journal of Sociology. 44 (6): 868–896. doi:10.1086/218177. S2CID 143603759.
  10. ^ Piaget, J. (1926) The Language and Thought of the Child. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
  11. ^ a b c Molouki, S., & Pronin, E. (2015). Self and other. In E. Borgida & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, Volume 1: Attitudes and Social Cognition. Washington, DC: APA. doi:10.1037/14341-013
  12. ^ Kretch, D.; Crutchfield, R. S. (1948). Theory and Problems of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw Hill. pp. 94.
  13. ^ Ichheiser, G. (1949). "Misunderstandings in Human Relations: A study in false social perception". American Journal of Sociology, Supplement to the September issue, pp. 1–72.
  14. ^ Asch, S. E. (1940). "Studies in the Principles of Judgments and Attitudes: II. Determination of Judgments by Group and by Ego Standards". The Journal of Social Psychology. 12 (2): 433–465. doi:10.1080/00224545.1940.9921487.
  15. ^ Asch, Solomon (1952). Social Psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc. pp. 46–47.
  16. ^ Hastorf, Albert H.; Cantril, Hadley (1954). "They saw a game; a case study". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 49 (1): 129–134. doi:10.1037/h0057880. PMID 13128974. S2CID 40577816.
  17. ^ Ross, Lee; Greene, David; House, Pamela (1977). "The "false consensus effect": An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 13 (3): 279–301. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(77)90049-x.
  18. ^ Vallone, Robert P.; Ross, Lee; Lepper, Mark R. (1985). "The hostile media phenomenon: Biased perception and perceptions of media bias in coverage of the Beirut massacre". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 49 (3): 577–585. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.577. PMID 4045697. S2CID 16311781.
  19. ^ Pronin, Emily; Lin, Daniel Y.; Ross, Lee (2002). "The Bias Blind Spot: Perceptions of Bias in Self Versus Others". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (3): 369–381. doi:10.1177/0146167202286008. S2CID 14259317.
  20. ^ Moskowitz, G.B. Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others. NY, NY: The Guilford Press, 2005.
  21. ^ Pronin, E., Puccio, C. T., & Ross, L. (2002). Understanding misunderstanding: Social psychological perspectives. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511808098.038
  22. ^ Jost, John T.; Major, Brenda (2001). The Psychology of Legitimacy: Emerging Perspectives on Ideology, Justice, and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-78699-7.
  23. ^ Ross, Lee (1995). "Reactive Devaluation in Negotiation and Conflict Resolution." In Kenneth Arrow, Robert Mnookin, Lee Ross, Amos Tversky, Robert B. Wilson (Eds.). Barriers to Conflict Resolution. New York: WW Norton & Co.

Further reading

  • Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1995). Psychological barriers to dispute resolution. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 27., (pp. 255–304). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press, ix, 317 pp. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60407-4
  • Lilienfeld, Scott O. (2010) 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology: Shattering Widespread Misconceptions About Human Behavior. Chichester, West Sussex; Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Pronin, Emily; Gilovich, Thomas; Ross, Lee (2004). "Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others". Psychological Review. 111 (3): 781–799. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.781. PMID 15250784.
  • Liberman, V.; et al. (2011). "Naïve realism and capturing the "wisdom of dyads"". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 48 (2): 507–512. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.016.
  • Keltner, Dacher; Robinson, Robert J. (1993). "Imagined Ideological Differences in Conflict Escalation and Resolution". International Journal of Conflict Management. 4 (3): 249–262. doi:10.1108/eb022728.
  • Liberman, Varda; Samuels, Steven M.; Ross, Lee (2004). "The Name of the Game: Predictive Power of Reputations versus Situational Labels in Determining Prisoner's Dilemma Game Moves". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (9): 1175–1185. doi:10.1177/0146167204264004. PMID 15359020. S2CID 19094952.
  • Ross, Lee (2014). "Barriers to agreement in the asymmetric Israeli–Palestinian conflict". Dynamics of Asymmetric Conflict. 7 (2–3): 120–136. doi:10.1080/17467586.2014.970565. S2CID 144631018.
  • Ross, Lee; Nisbett, Richard E. (2011). The Person and the Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology. Pinter & Martin Publishers. ISBN 978-1-905177-44-8.

Read other articles:

БеттборнBettborn   Країна  Франція Регіон Гранд-Ест  Департамент Мозель  Округ Саррбур-Шато-Сален Кантон Фенетранж Код INSEE 57071 Поштові індекси 57930 Координати 48°48′06″ пн. ш. 7°01′20″ сх. д.H G O Висота 232 - 322 м.н.р.м. Площа 6,57 км² Населення 380 (01-2020[1]) Густота 65,45 о

Bellardia trixago Біологічна класифікація Царство: Рослини (Plantae) Клада: Судинні рослини (Tracheophyta) Клада: Покритонасінні (Angiosperms) Клада: Евдикоти (Eudicots) Клада: Айстериди (Asterids) Порядок: Губоцвіті (Lamiales) Родина: Вовчкові (Orobanchaceae) Триба: Rhinantheae Рід: BellardiaAll. Вид: B. trixago Біноміальна назва B...

Citizen Holdings Co., Ltd.Kantor pusat Citizen Watch di TokyoNama asliシチズンホールディングス株式会社Nama latinShichizun Hōrudingusu Kabushiki-gaishaSebelumnyaShokosha Watch Research Institute (before May 28, 1930)JenisPublic (K.K.)Kode emitenTYO: 7762Nikkei 225 ComponentDidirikan1918; 105 tahun lalu (1918)KantorpusatNishitōkyō, Tokyo, JepangTokohkunciMakoto Umehara (Presiden)ProdukJam tangan dan komponennya, Peralatan informasi, Peralatan elektronik, Peralatan mesin ...

Artaxerxes I. Artaxerxes I. (altpersisch Rtachschaçā [r̩.ta.xʃa.tsaː], [ər.ta-], persisch اردشیر Ardaschīr [ærdæˈʃiːr]; altgriechisch Ἀρταξέρξης Artaxérxēs, späterer Beiname Μακρόχειρ Makrocheir „Langhand“) war von 465 v. Chr. bis zu seinem Tod im Dezember 424 v. Chr. persischer Großkönig. Das Ende seines Akzessionsjahres leitete am 1. Nisannu 464 v. Chr. automatisch sein erstes Regierungsjahr ein. Inha...

Hiroshige Goshiki (五色町, Goshiki-chō) was a town located in Tsuna District, Hyōgo Prefecture, Japan. As of 2003, the town had an estimated population of 11,143 and a density of 191.43 persons per km². The total area was 58.21 km². On February 11, 2006, Goshiki was merged into the expanded city of Sumoto. It was twinned with Kronstadt, Russia. [1] References ^ Prospects for Japan-Russia Cooperation in a Post-September II World Archived 2012-02-19 at the Wayback Machine Authori...

State highway in Mason County, Michigan, United States M-116M-116 highlighted in redRoute informationMaintained by MDOTLength7.018 mi[1] (11.294 km)Existedc. June 1928[2][3]–presentMajor junctionsSouth end US 10 in LudingtonNorth endLudington State Park entrance LocationCountryUnited StatesStateMichiganCountiesMason Highway system Michigan State Trunkline Highway System Interstate US State Byways ← M-115→ M-117 M-116 is a 7.018...

This article is an orphan, as no other articles link to it. Please introduce links to this page from related articles; try the Find link tool for suggestions. (August 2023) Phanephos Names IUPAC name (S)-(+)-4,12-Bis(diphenylphosphino)-[2.2]-paracyclophane; (R)-(−)-4,12-Bis(diphenylphosphino)-[2.2]-paracyclophane Other names (S)-Phanephos; (R)-Phanephos Identifiers CAS Number (S): 364732-88-7 Y(R): 192463-40-4 Y 3D model (JSmol) (S): Interactive image ChemSpider...

National monument of prehistoric mounds built by Native Americans, in Iowa, United States Effigy Mounds National MonumentIUCN category III (natural monument or feature)[1]Big Bear Mound at Effigy Mounds National MonumentShow map of IowaShow map of the United StatesLocationAllamakee / Clayton Counties, Iowa, USANearest cityMarquette, Iowa and Dubuque, IowaCoordinates43°05′20″N 91°11′08″W / 43.0888°N 91.1856°W / 43.0888; -91.1856Area9.78 km2...

Japanese swimmer Natsumi HoshiHoshi at Kazan, Russia 2015Personal informationNational team JapanBorn (1990-08-21) August 21, 1990 (age 33)Saitama, Japan[2]Height1.64 m (5 ft 5 in)[3]Weight54 kg (119 lb)[4]SportSportSwimmingStrokesButterflyClubMizuno[1]CoachYoshikatsu Harada[2]Norimasa Hirai (national)[1] Medal record Representing  Japan Olympic Games 2012 London 200 m butterfly 2016 Rio de Janeiro 200...

هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (فبراير 2020) نادي العينموسم 2019–20الرئيسمازن بن ردادالمدير الفنيالحبيب بن رمضانالملعبمدينة الملك سعود بن عبد العزيز الرياضيةدوري الدرجة الأولى السعوديالأولكأس خادم ا�...

1984 Indian filmVetriTheatrical release posterDirected byS. A. ChandrasekharWritten byS. N. SundarProduced byP. S. VeerappaStarringVijayakanthVijiCinematographyM. KesavanEdited byGautham RajuMusic byShankar–GaneshProductioncompanyP. S. V. PicturesRelease date 17 February 1984 (1984-02-17) Running time128 minutesCountryIndiaLanguageTamil Vetri (transl. Victory) is a 1984 Indian Tamil-language action crime film directed by S. A. Chandrasekhar. Vijayakanth plays the lead r...

American politician Jonathan Taylor UpdegraffMember of the U.S. House of Representativesfrom OhioIn officeMarch 4, 1879 – November 30, 1882Preceded byJames MonroeSucceeded byJoseph D. TaylorConstituency18th district (1879–1881)16th district (1881–1882)Member of the Ohio Senatefrom the 22nd districtIn officeJanuary 1, 1872 – January 4, 1874Preceded byJared DunbarSucceeded byJ. K. Rukenbrod Personal detailsBorn(1822-05-13)May 13, 1822Mount Pleasant, Ohio, U...

Artikel ini sebatang kara, artinya tidak ada artikel lain yang memiliki pranala balik ke halaman ini.Bantulah menambah pranala ke artikel ini dari artikel yang berhubungan atau coba peralatan pencari pranala.Tag ini diberikan pada November 2022. Józef KondratLahir(1902-03-03)3 Maret 1902Przemyśl, Austria-Hungaria (kini Polandia)Meninggal4 Agustus 1974(1974-08-04) (umur 72)Warsawa, PolandiaPekerjaanPemeranTahun aktif1933-1972 Makam Kondrat di Pemakaman Militer Powązki, Warsawa Jó...

Stadium located in Hershey, Pennsylvania, USA Hersheypark StadiumFormer namesHershey Sports StadiumLocationHershey, PennsylvaniaOwnerHershey Entertainment & Resorts CompanyOperatorHershey Entertainment & Resorts CompanyCapacity15,641 Soccer & Football 30,000 ConcertsSurfaceA-Turf Premier XPConstructionBroke ground1937Built1937-1939OpenedMay 18, 1939 (1939-05-18)[1]Expanded1940Construction cost$1.5 million[1]TenantsHershey Wildcats (A-League) (1997...

Writer Saleem Haddadسليم حدادBornSaleem Haddad1983 (age 39–40)Kuwait City, KuwaitNationalityBritish, Lebanese, JordanianNotable workGuapaWebsitesaleemhaddad.com Saleem Haddad (Arabic: سليم حداد; born 1983) is an author, filmmaker and aid worker of Iraqi-German and Palestinian-Lebanese descent, whose debut novel Guapa was published in 2016.[1] Early life Saleem Haddad was born in Kuwait City in 1983 to a Lebanese-Palestinian father and an Iraqi-German mother....

Spanningen tussen Servië en Kosovo Soort Asymmetrisch conflict Periode Juli 2022 - heden Partijen  Servië  Kosovo  NAVO Leiders Aleksandar Vučić Miloš Vučević  Vjosa Osmani Angelo Michele Ristuccia Plaats Grensregio Servië/Kosovo Casus belli Maatregelen betreffende kentekenplaten In het ​ groen Kosovo met daarboven in het ​ oranje Servië. Portaal    Kosovo De spanningen tussen Servië en Kosovo begonnen op 31 juli 2022, toen bekend werd dat K...

Cet armorial peut être amélioré car il comporte les défauts suivants : il comporte peu ou pas de sources. il reste des blasons à dessiner dans cet armorial. certaines figures sont encore dans un format bitmap et doivent être vectorisées. Vous pouvez partager vos connaissances en l’améliorant (comment ?) selon les recommandations du Projet Blasons. Cette page donne les armoiries (figures et blasonnements) des communes de la Vendée. Sur les autres projets Wikimedia : A...

Process of analyzing data produced by a test for DNA copy number variation Schematic karyogram of a typical human cell during the G0 phase in relation to the cell cycle, which is most common phase among human cells, and wherein there will be two pairs of each chromosome (designated 2n), each with one copy of each locus (for a total of 2c). In contrast, at center top it also shows the chromosome 3 pair in metaphase, which is among the phases occurring after DNA synthesis but before cell divisi...

Traditional dance in Zimbabwe Mbende Jerusarema danceMbende Jerusarema danceNative nameMbende JerusaremaEtymologyfast-running mouseGenreTraditional danceInstrument(s)Drum, Skins of animals used as skirtsInventorThe Zezuru people of Murehwa and Uzumba-Maramba-PfungweYearBefore 1890OriginZimbabwe Mbende Jerusarema dance is a prominent dance style among the Zezuru Shona of eastern Zimbabwe, particularly in the Murewa and Uzumba-Maramba-Pfungwe districts.[1][2] Traditional dances ...

Achille Emana Informasi pribadiNama lengkap Achille Emana EdzimbiTanggal lahir 5 Juni 1982 (umur 41)Tempat lahir Yaoundé, KamerunTinggi 180 m (590 ft 7 in)Posisi bermain Attacking midfielderInformasi klubKlub saat ini Al-HilalNomor 8Karier junior1997–1999 Babimbi Douala1999–2000 Valencia2000–2001 ToulouseKarier senior*Tahun Tim Tampil (Gol)2001–2008 Toulouse 231 (28)2008–2011 Betis 91 (34)2011– Al-Hilal 9 (4)Tim nasional‡2003– Kamerun 38 (6) * Penampilan d...