Act of analyzing from an alternative point of view
Perspective-taking is the act of perceiving a situation or understanding a concept from an alternative point of view, such as that of another individual.[1]
A vast amount of scientific literature suggests that perspective-taking is crucial to human development[2] and that it may lead to a variety of beneficial outcomes.[3][4] Perspective-taking may also be possible in some non-human animals.[5]
Both theory and research have suggested ages when children begin to perspective-take and how that ability develops over time.[2][6] Research suggests that certain people who have attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with comorbid conduct problems (such as Oppositional Defiant Disorder)[7][8] or autism[9] may have reduced ability to engage in perspective-taking.
Studies to assess the brain regions involved in perspective-taking suggest that several regions may be involved, including the prefrontal cortex and the precuneus.[10]
Perspective-taking takes place when an individual views a situation from another's point-of-view.[1][13] Perspective-taking has been defined along two dimensions: perceptual and conceptual.[14]
Perceptual perspective-taking is the ability to understand how another person experiences things through their senses (i.e. visually or auditorily).[14] Most of the literature devoted to perceptual perspective-taking focuses on visual perspective-taking: the ability to understand the way another person sees things in physical space.[6]
Conceptual perspective-taking is the ability to comprehend and take on the viewpoint of another person's psychological experience (i.e. thoughts, feelings, and attitudes).[14]
Related terms
Theory of mind
Theory of mind is the awareness that people have individual psychological states that differ from one another.[15] Within perspective-taking literature, the term perspective-taking and theory of mind are sometimes used interchangeably; some studies use theory of mind tasks in order to test if someone is engaging in perspective-taking.[16] The two concepts are related but different: theory of mind is the recognition that another person has different thoughts and feelings while perspective-taking is the ability to take on that other person's point of view.[11]
Empathy
Empathy has been defined as the ability to share the same emotions another person is having.[17] Empathy and perspective-taking have been studied together in a variety of ways. There are not always clear lines of distinction between empathy and perspective-taking; the two concepts are often studied in conjunction with one another and viewed as related and similar. Some research distinguishes the two concepts and points out their differences,[1] while other literature theorizes that perspective-taking is one component of empathy.[12][18]
In development
Visual
Studies have assessed the age at which humans are capable of visual perspective-taking, and have drawn different conclusions.
In 1956, Jean Piaget and Bärbel Inhelder conducted a study to assess the visual perspective-taking abilities of young children. This study has come to be known as the three mountain problem.[2] It found that by the ages of nine to ten, children can successfully complete the three mountain problem and they seem able to understand that when someone is standing in a different location (i.e. on a different mountain top) they have a different view. However, children ages eight and under struggled with this task.[2]
Since this classic study, a number of studies have suggested that visual perspective-taking may be possible earlier than the age of nine.[19][20] For example, a study that used a different method to assess visual perspective-taking suggested that children may be able to successfully visually perspective-take by the age of four and a half years old.[19] In this study, four-and-a-half-year-old children were able to understand that someone sitting closer to a picture would have a better view of that picture. However, these researchers found that children who were three and three-and-a-half years old struggled with this task which led them to conclude that the age range of three to four-and-a-half years old could be crucial in perspective-taking development.[19]
Developmental psychologist John H. Flavell suggested that there are two levels of visual perspective-taking that emerge as children develop:[6]
Level 1 perspective-taking is defined as the ability to understand that someone else may see things differently and to understand what another person can see in physical space.[6] For example, one could understand that while an object may be obstructing their own view, from where another person is standing they can see a cat in the room.
Level 2 perspective-taking is defined as the understanding that another person can see things differently in physical space and to understand how those objects are organized from that other person's point of view.[6] For example, a person can understand that from another person's point of view they can see a dog to the right but from their own point of view the dog is to the left.
Studies have examined when children are able to demonstrate level 1 and level 2 perspective-taking. These studies have shown that children at 24 months old[21][22] and 14 months old[23] may be able to engage in level 1 perspective-taking, and be able to understand various lines of sight depending on the position of a person.[24][25] Research also suggests that children can engage in level 2 perspective-taking as early as two and a half years old.[26]
Studies also suggest that visual perspective-taking ability improves from childhood to adulthood.[27] For example, in comparing six-year-olds, eight-year-olds, ten-year-olds, and adults (averaging 19 years old) researchers found that as people's age increases, visual perspective-taking tasks can be done with more accuracy and speed.[28]
Conceptual
In Piaget's theory of cognitive development, he suggests that perspective-taking begins in the concrete operational stage (third stage) which ranges from ages seven to twelve.[29] In this stage the idea of decentration is introduced as a cognitive ability (decentration is the ability to take into account the way others perceive various aspects of a given situation).[29]
Another developmental perspective-taking theory was created by Robert L. Selman and called social perspective-taking theory (or Role-taking theory). This theory suggests that there are five developmental stages involved in perspective-taking ranging from ages three to six (characterized by egocentrism or an inability to think of things from another's point of view) to teenagers and adults (who can understand another person's point of view and whose understanding is informed by recognizing another person's environment and culture).[30] The theory suggests that as humans mature from childhood to adulthood their ability to perspective-take improves. Studies by Selman and colleagues suggest that children can perspective-take in different ways at different ages.[31]
Other studies assess that children can begin to take on the viewpoint of another person, considering their feelings, thoughts, and attitudes, as four-year-olds.[14][32]
In adults
Although the distinction between visual and cognitive perspective-taking is important, some authors claim that all forms of perspective-taking rely on the same general ability, and different types of perspective-taking correlate.[33][34]
With respect to visual perspective-taking in adults, Tversky and Hard (2009) have shown that observers tend to use the point of view of another person when describing the spatial relations of objects.[35] Processing other perspectives may be spontaneous, and according to some studies even automatic.[36]
There are also cases where we spontaneously evaluate what another person is seeing, but we make systematic mistakes. A striking example is when a person in a scene is looking towards a mirror, we often interpret that as if they are seeing themselves even when the layout makes it impossible. This phenomenon is known as the Venus effect[37]
Brain regions
Visual
Visual perspective-taking studies that focus on brain regions are generally performed by collecting functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data while participants perform perspective-taking tasks. For example, a participant may be shown a picture of another person with objects around them and asked to take on the viewpoint of that person and indicate the number of objects they see (Level 1 visual perspective-taking) and if the objects are located to the right or left of the other person (level 2 visual perspective-taking).[10] While the participant is completing this task they are also having an fMRI scan.
A meta-analysis that looked at fMRI research on visual perspective-taking as of 2013 suggested that several areas of the brain have clustered activation during these perspective-taking tasks. These areas included the left prefrontal cortex, the precuneus, and the left cerebellum.[10] Studies suggest these areas of the brain are involved in decision-making[38] visual imagery,[39] and attention[40] respectively[clarification needed].
Conceptual
Research also suggests that multiple brain areas are involved in conceptual perspective-taking. Studies have been conducted by administering a positron emission tomography (PET) scan and asking participants to engage in perspective-taking tasks. For example, in one study, participants who were all medical students were asked to consider the knowledge base someone who was not in the medical field would have on a list of medical questions.[41]
Studies suggest that regions that are activated during cognitive perspective-taking include the right parietal lobe and the posterior cingulate cortex among others.[41][42] Some areas seem to be involved both when people imagine themselves and when they imagine the perspective of others. For example, when participants were asked to imagine themselves engaging in an activity versus imagining another person engaging in that activity the precuneus and the supplementary motor area (SMA) were activated, suggesting visual imagery and motor movement thoughts were involved in both tasks.[42]
Deficits
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
Research highlights that perspective-taking may be more difficult for some children who have attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) plus co-occurring conduct disorders. ADHD research has shown that children with this diagnosis demonstrate impairments in attention and communication:[7] They have a harder time taking on the viewpoint of others than children who do not.[8]
Autism
Evidence suggests that children with autism may be able to engage in visual perspective-taking but have difficulty engaging in conceptual perspective-taking.[43] For example, a study that compared perspective-taking scores in children who had been diagnosed with autism as compared to children who did not have this diagnosis found no significant difference in scores on level 1 and level 2 visual perspective-taking. However, the study found it was much harder for autistic children to engage in conceptual perspective-taking tasks.[9]
Some studies have explored potential interventions that could help improve perspective-taking abilities in children with autism. They suggest that video may help teach perspective-taking skills in such children.[44][16] One intervention study with autistic children found that showing the children a video of someone engaging in perspective-taking tasks and explaining their actions led to improved perspective-taking ability.[16]
Outcomes
An abundance of literature links perspective-taking abilities with other behaviors. Much of this literature focuses on conceptual perspective-taking.[14]
Benefit
Conceptual perspective-taking gives one the ability to better understand the reason behind another person's actions. This also helps one engage in social conversations in an acceptable and friendly way.
Empathy
Many studies associate perspective-taking with empathy. Psychologist Mark Davis suggested that empathy consists of multiple dimensions. To assess this, Davis developed the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI).[12] The IRI consists of four subscales: fantasy, empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking. The perspective-taking subscale asks participants to report how likely they are to engage in trying to see things from another person's point of view.[12] Studies using this widely cited measure found that perspective-taking is associated with many prosocial behaviors. One study, which assessed cross-cultural data in 63 countries using the IRI, concluded that perspective-taking and empathic concern was associated with volunteerism and agreeableness as well as self-esteem and life satisfaction.[45]
Research suggests that perspective-taking leads to empathic concern.[46] This research distinguishes between two different types of perspective-taking: thinking of how oneself would act, feel, and behave if placed in someone else's situation and thinking of the way that another person thinks, feels, and behaves in their own situation.[46] The results of this research reveals that thinking of how another person behaves and feels in their own situation leads to feelings of empathy. However, thinking of how one would behave in another person's situation leads to feelings of empathy as well as of distress.[46]
Research also finds that in negotiations, taking on the perspective of another person and empathizing with them may have differential outcomes[clarification needed]. One study found that people who engaged in perspective-taking were more effective in making a deal with another person and in finding innovative agreements that satisfied both parties, as compared to those who empathized with someone else.[1]
Sympathy and caring
Research reveals that perspective-taking is associated with sympathy toward others and prosocial behavior in children as young as 18 months old.[47] A study of sibling interactions found that toddlers who were older siblings were more likely to help take care of their younger siblings when they[clarification needed] demonstrated higher perspective-taking abilities.[48]
Creativity
Perspective-taking is also associated with creativity. It increases the amount of creative ideas generated in team activities.[49] One study suggests that perspective-taking leads to more creative and innovative ideas particularly in participants who are internally driven to complete a task.[4]
Bias and stereotype reduction
Many studies find potential benefits of perspective-taking on the perceptions of outgroup members[clarification needed]. Studies on perspective-taking and bias and stereotyping are generally done by asking participants to take the perspective of another person who is different from them in certain domains (i.e. asking young adult participants to take on the perspective of an elderly person or asking White participants to take on the perspective of a Black person seen in a photograph or video).[3] Such studies show that perspective-taking can lead to reduced stereotyping of outgroup members,[50] improved attitudes towards others,[51] and increased helping behavior of outgroup members[clarification needed]. Perspective-taking can lead to reduced in-group favoritism.[50] Research on implicit (or unconscious) biases found that perspective-taking can reduce implicit bias scores (as measured by the Implicit-association test) as well as more recognition of subtle discrimination.[clarification needed][3]
In disagreements
Research into differences between having a conversation with someone whom you agree with versus with someone with whom you disagree finds that participants who disagreed had enhanced perspective-taking ability and could better remember the conversation.[52]
Drawbacks
Some researchers suggest drawbacks to perspective-taking. For example, studies found that asking people to engage in perspective-taking tasks can lead to increased stereotyping of the target if the target is deemed as having more stereotypic qualities[53] and as adopting stereotypic behaviors of outgroup members.[54]
Other animals
Studies to assess if nonhuman animals can successfully engage in perspective-taking have not drawn consistent conclusions. Many such studies assess perspective-taking by training animals on specific tasks or by measuring how consistently animals follow the eye gaze of humans. Being able to successfully follow another's eye gaze could indicate that the animal is aware that the human is seeing and paying attention to something that is different from what they see.[55]
A study of spider monkeys and capuchin monkeys found that these primates successfully performed eye gazing tasks. This led researchers to conclude that the monkeys demonstrated some ability to consider another person's viewpoint.[55] However, another study found that Rhesus monkeys were unsuccessful at such eye gazing tasks.[56]
Studies suggest that dogs have complex social understanding.[57] In one study, researchers told a dog it was not allowed to eat a treat and then placed the treat in a location that the dog could reach.[5] Dogs were more likely to eat the treat after being instructed not to if there was a barrier that hid the dog from the instructor. Dogs were less likely to eat the treat if the barrier was of smaller size or had a window in it. This study also showed that dogs struggled in other tasks that focused on the dog's own visual attention. These researchers suggest that this study provides evidence that dogs may be aware of other's visual perspectives.[5]
^ abcdGalinsky AD, Maddux WW, Gilin D, White JB (April 2008). "Why it pays to get inside the head of your opponent: the differential effects of perspective taking and empathy in negotiations". Psychological Science. 19 (4): 378–84. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02096.x. PMID18399891. S2CID34508802.
^ abcdPiagetJ, Inhelder B, Langdon FJ, Lunzer JL (1967). The child's conception of space. New York. ISBN978-0393004083. OCLC444733.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
^ abcTodd AR, Bodenhausen GV, Richeson JA, Galinsky AD (June 2011). "Perspective taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (6): 1027–42. CiteSeerX10.1.1.470.8388. doi:10.1037/a0022308. PMID21381852.
^ abcBräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2004). "Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 88 (3–4): 299–317. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2004.03.004.
^ abcdeFlavell JH (1977). "The development of knowledge about visual perception". Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 25: 43–76. PMID753993.
^ abNilsen ES, Fecica AM (2011-03-01). "A model of communicative perspective-taking for typical and atypical populations of children". Developmental Review. 31 (1): 55–78. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.001.
^ abMarton I, Wiener J, Rogers M, Moore C, Tannock R (January 2009). "Empathy and social perspective taking in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 37 (1): 107–18. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9262-4. PMID18712471. S2CID32318195.
^ abReed T, Peterson C (December 1990). "A comparative study of autistic subjects' performance at two levels of visual and cognitive perspective taking". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 20 (4): 555–67. doi:10.1007/BF02216060. PMID2279973. S2CID31553257.
^ abHarwood MD, Farrar MJ (2006-06-01). "Conflicting emotions: The connection between affective perspective taking and theory of mind". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 24 (2): 401–418. doi:10.1348/026151005X50302. ISSN2044-835X.
^ abcdDavis MH (1983). "Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 44 (1): 113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. hdl:10983/25968.
^ abcCharlop-Christy MH, Daneshvar S (2003). "Using Video Modeling to Teach Perspective Taking to Children with Autism". Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 5 (1): 12–21. doi:10.1177/10983007030050010101. S2CID144952720.
^ abcFlavell JH, Flavell EF, Green FL, Wilcox SA (1980). "Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Effect of observer's distance from target on perceptual clarity of target". Developmental Psychology. 16 (1): 10–12. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.16.1.10.
^Newcombe N, Huttenlocher J (1992). "Children's early ability to solve perspective-taking problems". Developmental Psychology. 28 (4): 635–643. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.635.
^Moll H, Tomasello M (2006-09-01). "Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 24 (3): 603–613. doi:10.1348/026151005x55370. ISSN2044-835X.
^Sodian B, Thoermer C, Metz U (March 2007). "Now I see it but you don't: 14-month-olds can represent another person's visual perspective". Developmental Science. 10 (2): 199–204. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00580.x. PMID17286844.
^Selman RL (1971). "Taking Another's Perspective: Role-Taking Development in Early Childhood". Child Development. 42 (6): 1721–1734. doi:10.2307/1127580. JSTOR1127580.
^Selman RL, Byrne DF (1974). "A Structural-Developmental Analysis of Levels of Role Taking in Middle Childhood". Child Development. 45 (3): 803–806. doi:10.2307/1127850. JSTOR1127850. PMID4143833.
^Samson, D, Apperly, IA, Braithwaite, JJ, Andrews, BJ, BodleyScott, SE (2010). "Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 36 (5): 1255–1266. doi:10.1037/a0018729. PMID20731512.
^Bertamini, M, Latto, R, Spooner, A (2003). "The venus effect: People's understanding of mirror reflections in paintings". Perception. 32 (5): 593–599. doi:10.1068/p3418. PMID12854645. S2CID33410814.
^Barraclough DJ, Conroy ML, Lee D (April 2004). "Prefrontal cortex and decision making in a mixed-strategy game". Nature Neuroscience. 7 (4): 404–10. doi:10.1038/nn1209. PMID15004564. S2CID323396.
^ abRuby P, Decety J (June 2003). "What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 17 (11): 2475–80. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02673.x. PMID12814380. S2CID16125688.
^ abRuby P, Decety J (May 2001). "Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency". Nature Neuroscience. 4 (5): 546–50. doi:10.1038/87510. PMID11319565. S2CID3116452.
^Baron-Cohen S (September 1988). "Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: cognitive or affective?". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 18 (3): 379–402. doi:10.1007/bf02212194. PMID3049519. S2CID2050249.
^ abcBatson CD, Early S, Salvarani G (2016-07-02). "Perspective Taking: Imagining How Another Feels Versus Imaging How You Would Feel". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 23 (7): 751–758. doi:10.1177/0146167297237008. S2CID145333456.
^Vaish A, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (March 2009). "Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers". Developmental Psychology. 45 (2): 534–43. CiteSeerX10.1.1.215.3105. doi:10.1037/a0014322. PMID19271837.
^Stewart RB, Marvin RS (1984). "Sibling Relations: The Role of Conceptual Perspective-Taking in the Ontogeny of Sibling Caregiving". Child Development. 55 (4): 1322–1332. doi:10.2307/1130002. JSTOR1130002.
^Hoever IJ, van Knippenberg D, van Ginkel WP, Barkema HG (September 2012). "Fostering team creativity: perspective taking as key to unlocking diversity's potential". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 97 (5): 982–96. doi:10.1037/a0029159. PMID22774764. S2CID23615180.
^ abGalinsky AD, Moskowitz GB (2000). "Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (4): 708–724. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.708. PMID10794375.
^Vescio TK, Sechrist GB, Paolucci MP (2003-07-01). "Perspective taking and prejudice reduction: the mediational role of empathy arousal and situational attributions". European Journal of Social Psychology. 33 (4): 455–472. doi:10.1002/ejsp.163. ISSN1099-0992.
^Tjosvold D, Johnson DW (1977). "Effects of controversy on cognitive perspective taking". Journal of Educational Psychology. 69 (6): 679–685. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.69.6.679.
^Galinsky AD, Wang CS, Ku G (August 2008). "Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 95 (2): 404–19. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.2.404. PMID18665710.
^ abAmici F, Aureli F, Visalberghi E, Call J (November 2009). "Spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) follow gaze around barriers: evidence for perspective taking?". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 123 (4): 368–74. doi:10.1037/a0017079. PMID19929105. S2CID14267790.
^Anderson JR, Montant M, Schmitt D (August 1996). "Rhesus monkeys fail to use gaze direction as an experimenter-given cue in an object-choice task". Behavioural Processes. 37 (1): 47–55. doi:10.1016/0376-6357(95)00074-7. PMID24897158. S2CID19504558.
^Agnetta B, Hare B, Tomasello M (2000-12-01). "Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use". Animal Cognition. 3 (2): 107–112. doi:10.1007/s100710000070. ISSN1435-9448. S2CID24594183.
2003 American action film directed by Andrzej Bartkowiak For other uses, see Cradle to the Grave (disambiguation). Cradle 2 the GraveTheatrical release posterDirected byAndrzej BartkowiakScreenplay byJohn O'BrienChanning GibsonStory byJohn O'BrienProduced byJoel SilverStarring Jet Li DMX Anthony Anderson Kelly Hu Tom Arnold Mark Dacascos Gabrielle Union CinematographyDaryn OkadaEdited byDerek BrechinMusic byJohn FrizzellDamon Grease BlackmanProductioncompanySilver PicturesDistributed byWarner...
I Nengah Tamba Bupati Jembrana ke-14PetahanaMulai menjabat 26 Februari 2021PresidenJoko WidodoGubernurWayan KosterWakilI Gede Ngurah Patriana Krisna PendahuluI Putu ArthaPenggantiPetahana Informasi pribadiLahir22 Maret 1964 (umur 60)Jembrana, Bali, IndonesiaKebangsaanIndonesiaPartai politikPNBK (2004-2006) Demokrat (2006-sekarang)Suami/istriGusti Ayu Ketut CandrawatiAnak3Alma materUniversitas UdayanaPekerjaanDirektur, PolitikusSunting kotak info • L • B I Nengah Tam...
Diana DeGette Diana Louise DeGette (/dɪˈɡɛt/; lahir 29 Juli 1957) adalah seorang politikus Amerika Serikat yang menjabat sebagai anggota DPR sejak 1997. Ia berasal dari Partai Demokrat. Pranala luar Wikimedia Commons memiliki media mengenai Diana DeGette. Congresswoman Diana DeGette official U.S. House website Diana DeGette for Congress Diarsipkan 2021-01-16 di Wayback Machine. Diana DeGette di Curlie (dari DMOZ) Biografi di Biographical Directory of the United States Congress Catatan sua...
Groupe La France insoumise - Nouvelle Union populaire écologique et sociale Chambre Assemblée nationale Législature(s) XVe et XVIe (Cinquième République) Fondation 27 juin 2017 Partis membres La France insoumiseParti de gaucheGauche écosocialistePicardie deboutRézistan's Égalité 974Révolution écologique pour le vivantParti ouvrier indépendantPéyi-A Présidente Mathilde Panot Représentation 75 / 577 Positionnement Gauche radicale[1],[2],[3],[4] à extrême gauche[5],...
Component of the US Army and military of the state of Iowa Headquarters, State Area CommandIowa National GuardIowa Army National Guard Headquarters DUICountryUnited StatesAllegianceIowaBranchNational GuardTypeARNG Headquarters CommandPart ofIowa National GuardGarrison/HQJohnston, IowaMilitary unit The Iowa Army National Guard is a state agency of the State of Iowa, with significant funding from the Federal Government of the United States; and a reserve component of the United States Army...
كأس فيتنام 2017 تفاصيل الموسم كأس فيتنام البلد فيتنام البطل نادي سونغ لام نغي أن عدد المشاركين 21 كأس فيتنام 2016 كأس فيتنام 2018 تعديل مصدري - تعديل كأس فيتنام 2017 (بالفيتنامية: Giải bóng đá Cúp Quốc gia 2017) هو موسم من كأس فيتنام. كان عدد الأندية المشاركة فيه 21، وف...
Temple at Angkor, Cambodia This article is about Preah Khan in Angkor, near Siem Reap. For Preah Khan (of) Kompong Svay, in Preah Vinear province, 100 km (62 mi) at east of Angkor, see Preah Khan Kompong Svay. Preah KhanReligionAffiliationHinduismDeityVishnuLocationLocationAngkorCountryCambodiaLocation in CambodiaGeographic coordinates13°27′43″N 103°52′18″E / 13.4619594°N 103.8715911°E / 13.4619594; 103.8715911ArchitectureTypeKhmerCreatorJayavarma...
Elezioni parlamentari in Austria del 2002 Stato Austria Data 24 novembre Assemblea Nationalrat Leader Wolfgang Schüssel Alfred Gusenbauer Jörg Haider Liste ÖVP SPÖ FPÖ Voti 2.076.83342,30% 1.792.49936,51% 491.32810,01% Seggi 79 / 183 69 / 183 18 / 183 Distribuzione del voto per stato ÖVP SPÖ FPÖ Verdi Cancellieri Wolfgang Schüssel 1999 2006 Le elezioni parlam...
U.S. atomic bomb type used at Nagasaki, 1945 This article is about the World War II nuclear weapon. For other uses, see Fat Man (disambiguation). Fat Man Replica of the original Fat Man bombTypeNuclear fission gravity bombPlace of originUnited StatesProduction historyDesignerLos Alamos LaboratoryProduced1945–1949No. built120SpecificationsMass10,300 pounds (4,670 kg)Length128 inches (3.3 m)Diameter60 inches (1.5 m)FillingPlutoniumFilling weight6.4 kgBlast...
Louis I, Count of NeversCount Louis (right)Born1272Died(1322-07-22)22 July 1322ParisNoble familyHouse of DampierreSpouse(s)Joan, Countess of RethelIssue Joanna, Duchess of Brittany Louis I, Count of Flanders FatherRobert III, Count of FlandersMotherYolande II, Countess of Nevers Louis I (1272 – 22 July 1322) was suo jure Count of Nevers and jure uxoris Count of Rethel. Louis was a son of Robert III, Count of Flanders,[1] and Yolande, Countess of Nevers.[2] He succeeded h...
Neasden DepotThe inside of the depot in 1988LocationLocationNeasden, London, EnglandHistoryOpened1882 South end of Neasden Depot, photo taken from the North Circular Road (A406) bridge Neasden Depot is a London Underground depot located in Neasden in the London Borough of Brent, between Neasden and Wembley Park stations on the Metropolitan line. It is the largest depot on the London Underground, and is currently responsible for maintenance and overhaul of the 191 S Stock trains used on the C...
Election for the governorship of the U.S. state of Minnesota 1906 Minnesota gubernatorial election ← 1904 November 6, 1906 1908 → Nominee John Albert Johnson A. L. Cole Party Democratic Republican Popular vote 168,480 96,162 Percentage 60.93% 34.78% County resultsJohnson: 40–50% 50–60% 60–70% 70–80%Cole: ...
2007 book by Anne Phillips Multiculturalism without Culture AuthorAnne PhillipsCountryUnited KingdomLanguageEnglishSubjectMulticulturalism, feminism, political philosophyGenreNon-fictionPublisherPrinceton University PressPublication dateMay 21, 2007Media typeHardbackPages216 ppISBN978-0-691-12944-0 (hardback)OCLC71275406Dewey Decimal305.4809182/1 22LC ClassHQ1161 .P452 2007 Multiculturalism without Culture is a book written by Anne Phillips.[1][2] The topic of multic...
المعهد العالي للعلوم التطبيقية والتكنولوجيا شعار المعهد العالي للعلوم التطبيقية والتكنولوجيا معلومات التأسيس 1983 النوع جامعة حكومية الموقع الجغرافي إحداثيات 33°33′15″N 36°18′44″E / 33.5542°N 36.3123°E / 33.5542; 36.3123 الشارع مساكن برزة (دمشق) سيف الدولة -كلية العلوم(حل...
Use of natural catalysts to perform chemical transformations This article is about natural catalysts used to perform chemical transformations. For large biological molecule that acts as a catalyst, see Biocatalysts. Three dimensional structure of an enzyme. Biocatalysis utilizes these biological macromolecules to catalyze small molecule transformations. Biocatalysis refers to the use of living (biological) systems or their parts to speed up (catalyze) chemical reactions. In biocatalytic proce...
Tomoko Kanazawa金澤 朋子Informasi latar belakangNama lainKanatomo (かなともcode: ja is deprecated )Lahir2 Juli 1995 (umur 29)AsalSaitama, JepangGenreJ-popPekerjaanPenyanyiTahun aktif2012-2022Labelhachama, UP-FRONT WORKSArtis terkaitJuice=Juice, Hello! Pro KenshuuseiSitus webHello! Project Tomoko Kanazawa (金澤 朋子code: ja is deprecated , Kanazawa Tomoko, lahir 2 Juli 1995) adalah seorang mantan penyanyi pop Jepang. Dia adalah anggota dan wakil pemimpin grup idola Juice=Juice...
Medi 1 TVLogo de Medi 1 TVCaractéristiquesCréation 1er décembre 2006Slogan Un nouveau regard sur l'informationLangue Arabe et françaisPays MarocStatut Thématique internationale publiqueSiège social Zone Franche de Tanger - Lot 31BP 2397 TangerAncien nom Medi 1 Sat (2006-2010)Site web www.medi1tv.comDiffusionNumérique DVB-T TNT Maroc chaine 7Satellite Eutelsat 21B 21,6° EstEutelsat 7WA 7° OuestNilesat 201 7° OuestArabsat Badr 5 26° EstCâble NC Numéricable-SFR : Chaîne n° 40...
Parlement italien(it) Parlamento italiano XIXe législature Assemblée constituante Emblème de l'Italie.Présentation Type Bicaméral Chambres Chambre des députésSénat de la République Création 1948 Lieu Rome Durée du mandat 5 ans Présidence Chambre Lorenzo Fontana (Lega) Élection 14 octobre 2022 Sénat Ignazio La Russa (FdI) Élection 13 octobre 2022 Structure Membres 600 :400 députés200 sénateurs[a] Composition actuelle.Données clés Groupes politiques(Cham...