Evolutionary psychology and culture

Evolutionary psychology has traditionally focused on individual-level behaviors, determined by species-typical psychological adaptations. Considerable work, though, has been done on how these adaptations shape and, ultimately govern, culture (Tooby and Cosmides, 1989).[1] Tooby and Cosmides (1989) argued that the mind consists of many domain-specific psychological adaptations, some of which may constrain what cultural material is learned or taught. As opposed to a domain-general cultural acquisition program, where an individual passively receives culturally-transmitted material from the group, Tooby and Cosmides (1989), among others, argue that: "the psyche evolved to generate adaptive rather than repetitive behavior, and hence critically analyzes the behavior of those surrounding it in highly structured and patterned ways, to be used as a rich (but by no means the only) source of information out of which to construct a 'private culture' or individually tailored adaptive system; in consequence, this system may or may not mirror the behavior of others in any given respect." (Tooby and Cosmides 1989).[1]

Epidemiological culture

The Epidemiology of representations, or cultural epidemiology, is a broad framework for understanding cultural phenomena by investigating the distribution of mental representations in and through populations.[2] The theory of cultural epidemiology was largely developed by Dan Sperber to study society and cultures. The theory has implications for psychology and anthropology. Mental representations are transferred from person to person through cognitive causal chains. Sperber (2001) identified three different, yet interrelated, cognitive causal chains, outlined in Table 1. A cognitive causal chain (CCC) links a perception to an evolved, domain-specific response or process. For example:

On October 31, at 7:30 p.m., Mrs. Jones’s doorbell rings. Mrs. Jones hears the doorbell, and assumes that there is somebody at the door. She remembers it is Halloween: she enjoyed receiving treats as a child, and now, as an adult, she enjoys giving them. She guesses that there must be children at the door ready to trick-or-treat, and that, if she opens, she will be able to give them the candies she has bought for the occasion. Mrs. Jones decides to open the door, and does so.[2]

Social Cognitive Causal Chains (SCCC) are inter-individual CCCs. Here, a CCC is used as an act of communication between people (i.e. the mental representation is shared between multiple people). Elaborating on the previous example:

On October 31, at 7:30 p.m., Mrs. Jones’s doorbell rings. Mrs. Jones hears the doorbell, and assumes that there is somebody at the door. She remembers it is Halloween: she enjoyed receiving treats as a child, and now, as an adult, she enjoys giving them. She guesses that there must be children at the door ready to trick-or-treat, and that, if she opens, she will be able to give them the candies she has bought for the occasion. Mrs. Jones decides to open the door, and does so.[2]

Cultural Cognitive Causal Chains (CCCCs) are those SCCCs which are shared and reproduced widely among a population.

Cognitive causal chain (CCC)

A causal chain where each causal link instantiates a semantic relationship

Social cognitive causal chain (SCCC)

A CCC that extends over several individuals

Cultural cognitive causal chain (CCCC)

A social CCC that stabilizes mental representations and public productions in a population and its environment

Table 1: Overview of cognitive causal chains constituting the epidemiology of representations (from [2]).

The stability and longevity of these representations relies on their relevance and domain-specificity. This separates the epidemiology of representations from other evolutionary accounts of cultural transmission, namely memetics.[3]

Culture and other evolutionary disciplines

Evolutionary psychology and cultural evolution

Evolutionary psychological accounts of culture, especially cultural epidemiology, may seem at odds with other disciplines in psychology and anthropology, especially research in cultural evolution (e.g. Boyd and Richerson, 1985, 2005).[4][5] Cultural evolution, the domain of research focused on how culture changes through time due to different individual transmission mechanisms and population-level effects, often uses models derived from population genetics, in which agents are passive recipients of cultural traits (e.g. Bentley et al., 2004[6]). While evolutionary psychology has focused on how behavior may result from species-typical psychological programs which led to greater fitness in ancestral environments, this work has been criticized for neglecting how behavior may be shaped by culturally transmitted information. In contrast to genetic programs, cultural evolution investigates how culture itself may evolve (Mesoudi, 2009[7]). Gene-culture coevolution studies how culture and genetic evolution influence each other, ultimately shaping behavior, as well. Cultural evolution and evolutionary psychology may not be as disparate as one may think, though. Rather than a passive receptacle of cultural material, Boyd and Richerson (1985, 2005)[4][5] suggest that our minds consist of psychological mechanisms which direct our attention to and imitate cultural traits depending on the frequency of that trait, the content of it, who carries it, etc. In this way, cultural evolution may be viewed as a particular kind of “scaling up” from the individual-level processes to population-level ones. In neither account of culture is the mind a blank slate or empty bin for cultural material (Tooby and Cosmides, 1992[8]). Some evolutionary psychologists have suggested that culturally-transmitted culture may pass through a number of lenses and filters shaped by natural selection to catch fitness-relevant information and behaviors; these filters may change due to local environmental conditions, sex, and other factors (Gangestad et al., 2006[9]). Boyer (2000)[10] argued that evolutionary psychology, and anthropology in general, should investigate how these cognitive predispositions to cultural material affect their representation in following generations. This research program may help to unify the two fields (Boyer, 2000). Others have suggested that evolutionary psychology, human behavioral ecology, and cultural evolution all fit nicely with each other, ultimately answering different questions on human behavioral diversity; gene-culture coevolution may be at odds with other research programs though by placing more emphasis on socially-learned behavior and its influence on human evolution (Brown et al., 2011).[11]

Evolutionary psychology and human behavioral ecology

Human Behavioral Ecology (HBE) studies how ecology and social factors shape human behavioral variability. A key assumption of human behavioral ecology is that individuals will act in response to environmental factors in ways that enhance their fitness.[12] While both HBE and evolutionary psychology are both concerned with fitness benefits and natural selection, HBE is not typically concerned with mechanisms or how humans display adaptive behaviors in novel environments.[13] If the human mind took its present form and evolved in the African Pleistocene, why should one expect to behave adaptively in the present and in so many new environments?[14] Human Behavioral Ecologists may respond by stating that humans evolved as ecological generalists and display a wide range of variable behaviors to maximize fitness.[15] Where cooperation and food-sharing among extant hunter-gatherers may be seen as the result of kin-selection psychological mechanisms by an evolutionary psychologist, a human behavioral ecologist may attribute this behavior to optimal risk-reduction strategies and reciprocal altruism.[16][17] For Human Behavioral Ecology, cultural behaviors should tend towards optimality (i.e. higher fitness) in their current environment. For evolutionary psychology, cultural behaviors are the result of psychological mechanisms which were selected for in ancestral environments.

Evoked and transmitted culture

The difference between cultural behaviors acquired through selective transmission mechanisms and behaviors resulting from psychological adaptations has resulted in some researchers differentiating between evoked and transmitted culture (Tooby and Cosmides, 1992[8]). Evoked cultural behaviors are those that are the outputs of shared psychological mechanisms in response to local environmental cues[8] Fessler et al., 2015[18]). Transmitted cultural behaviors are those behaviors which are learned from one’s social group, regardless of environment. As such, investigating evoked culture may well rest in the domain of evolutionary psychology and transmitted culture studied by culture evolution, social psychology, and other disciplines. Differentiating between the two is more difficult than it may appear, however. For example, given the level of cross-cultural variation in human societies, variation between any two cultures may result from separate histories and cultural evolutionary pathways or different evoked cultures, based on different local environmental cues, while uniformity across cultures could result from convergent cultural evolution or similarly functioning psychological adaptations (Fessler et al., 2015[18]). Specifically, if two cultures exhibit similar behaviors to avoid pathogens, can one differentiate between either the two groups independently inventing similar behaviors or the groups displaying the same underlying psychological adaptation for pathogen avoidance? As well, the issue of behavioral variation and transmitted culture may be seen as a major point of contention between evolutionary psychology and other disciplines, especially gene-culture coevolution. Evolutionary psychology and human behavioral ecology both admit to cultural transmission as a source of behavioral variation but do not see it as an evolutionary process or its potential to have significantly affected human evolution. Gene-culture coevolution would argue that culture traits may have altered genetic change and selection pressures, ultimately affecting cognition. However, as Brown et al. (2011)[11] states:"Whether human behavioural ecologists and evolutionary psychologists are willing to accommodate the idea that some portion of human behavioural diversity could result from genetic differences that have arisen via selection pressures imposed by socially transmitted behaviour remains to be seen."

While this may difficult, some evolutionary psychologists, such as Gangestad, Haselton, and Buss (2006)[9] have argued that the future of the discipline rests on unifying transmitted and evoked culture. In order to do so, the authors suggest that a more comprehensive definition of culture needs to be developed since different cultural phenomena need different kinds of analysis to investigate their effects. As they state, “Culture, however, is not a "thing" with singularity; it's an umbrella concept subsuming a collection of extraordinarily varied phenomena, each of which requires scientific analysis” (Gangestad et al., 2006).[9] Ultimately, the issues of behavioral variation through evoked or transmitted culture must rest on empirical evidence.

Psychology, culture, and human evolution

While most researchers would accept the notion that psychological adaptations, shaped by natural selection, underlie culture, some would stress the strength of cultural behaviors on shaping selection pressures (Boyd and Richerson, 2005)[5] or emphasize cultural niche construction (Laland et al., 2000).[19] Still others have argued for a kind of cognitive niche construction, where culture changes the selection pressures on cognition, resulting in emergent psychological modules (e.g. Wheeler and Clark, 2008[20]). Wheeler and Clark (2008) have named this interplay between genes, culture, and embodiment the “triple helix.” The authors suggest that self-created environmental structures and reliance on culturally-transmitted information selected for cognitive modules for continual bootstrapping and increases in computational complexity. Their argument relies on cognitive niche construction, where culturally-learned behaviors create a space for new feedback cycles. By shaping the physical space around oneself, for example, the culturally-transmitted practice transform problem solving for new forms of thought. The feedback cycle, as they and others argue, will alter selection pressures for cognitive developmental programs which bolster these abilities.[20][21] As they state, “Triple helix models of mind recognize the role of genetic biases in sculpting key developmental trajectories, and the resulting space both for strong forms of genetically specified cognitive modularity and for weaker forms of emergent modularity resulting from trajectories marked by multiple bouts of culturally scaffolded experience and the self- selection of environments” (Wheeler and Clark, 2008.[20] Other authors have suggested that borrowing methods of dynamical systems analysis may help unravel this tangled web of genes, cognition, and culture (Kenrick et al., 2003[22]). The answers to these type of questions have impacts on anthropology, social psychology, and other behavioral sciences (Mesoudi, 2009[7]).

Evolved responses to intergroup conflict

War and intergroup conflict is a strong evolutionary factor. The so-called regality theory describes how human psychological traits are responding to either a hostile or a peaceful environment. Humans show authoritarian reactions and preferences for a strong leader in case of war or perceived collective danger. The evolutionary function of this so-called regal reaction is that it allows a strong leader to solve the collective action problem in war and suppress free riding. The whole society and culture is shaped by the regal psychological reactions of its members. A regal society is characterized by an authoritarian and hierarchical organization, strict discipline, intolerance, and xenophobia.

The opposite of the regal reaction is called kungic. The kungic reaction is seen when a group of people are living in peace and security. People will not tolerate a tyrannical leader who can exploit all resources to enrich himself. Instead, people will prefer an egalitarian and tolerant society when they live in an environment of collective safety.[23]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Tooby, J.; Cosmides, L. (1989). "Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, part I. Theoretical considerations". Ethology and Sociobiology. 10: 29–49. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(89)90012-5.
  2. ^ a b c d Sperber, D. (2001). "Conceptual tools for a natural science of society and culture (Radcliffe-Brown Lecture in Social Anthropology 1999)". Proceedings of the British Academy. 111: 297–317.
  3. ^ Sperber, D. (2000). Aunger, Robert (ed.). Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science. Oxford.
  4. ^ a b Boyd, R.; Richerson, P.J. (1988). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0226069333.
  5. ^ a b c Boyd, R.; Richerson, P.J. (2005). The Origin and Evolution of Cultures. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0195181456.
  6. ^ Bentley, R.A.; Hahn, M.W.; Shennan, S.J. (2004). "Random drift and culture change". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 271 (1547): 1443–1450. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2746. PMC 1691747. PMID 15306315.
  7. ^ a b Mesoudi, A. (2009). "How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa". Psychological Review. 116 (4): 929–952. doi:10.1037/a0017062. PMID 19839691.
  8. ^ a b c Tooby, J.; Cosmides, L. (1995). Barkow, J.H.; Tooby, J.; Cosmides, L. (eds.). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0195101072.
  9. ^ a b c Gangestad, S.W.; Haselton, M.G.; Buss, D.M. (2006). "Authors' Response: Toward an Integrative Understanding of Evoked and Transmitted Culture: The Importance of Specialized Psychological Design". Psychological Inquiry. 17 (2): 138–151. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1702_3. S2CID 448323.
  10. ^ Boyer, P. (2000). "Evolutionary Psychology and Cultural Transmission". American Behavioral Scientist. 43 (6): 987–1000. doi:10.1177/00027640021955711. S2CID 144440417.
  11. ^ a b Brown, G.R.; Dickins, T.E.; Sear, R.; Laland, K.N. (2011). "Evolutionary Accounts of Human Behavioural Diversity". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. 366 (1563): 1029–1046. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0267. PMC 3013476. PMID 21199836.
  12. ^ Laland, K.N.; Brown, G.R. (2006). "Niche Construction, Human Behavior, and the Adaptive-Lag Hypothesis". Evolutionary Anthropology. 15 (3): 95–104. doi:10.1002/evan.20093.
  13. ^ Symons, D. Crawford, C.; Smith, M.; Krebs, D. (eds.). Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum.
  14. ^ Tooby, J.; Cosmides, L. (1990). "The Past Explains the Present: Emotional Adaptations and the Structure of Ancestral Environments". Ethology and Sociobiology. 11: 375–424. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-z.
  15. ^ Smith, E.A.; Wishnie, M. (2000). "Conservation and Subsistence in Small-scale Societies". Annual Review of Anthropology. 29: 493–524. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.29.1.493. S2CID 7165162.
  16. ^ Hawkes, K.; O'Connell, J.F.; Blurton Jones, N. (2001). "Hadza Meat Sharing". Evolution and Human Behavior. 22 (2): 113–142. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00066-0. PMID 11282309.
  17. ^ Kaplan, H.S.; Hill, K. (1985). "Food-sharing Among the Ache: Tests of Explanatory Hypotheses". Current Anthropology. 26: 223–245. doi:10.1086/203251. S2CID 144447219.
  18. ^ a b Fessler, D.M.T.; Clark, J.A.; Clint, E.K. (2015-11-02). Buss, D.M. (ed.). The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Wiley. pp. 1029–1046. ISBN 978-1-118-76399-5.
  19. ^ Laland, K.N.; Odling-Smee, J.; Feldman, M.W. (2000). "Niche Construction, Biological Evolution, and Cultural Change". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (1): 131–146. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00002417. PMID 11303338. S2CID 13893525.
  20. ^ a b c Wheeler, M.; Clark, A. (2008). "Culture, embodiment and genes: unravelling the triple helix". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 363 (1509): 3563–3575. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0135. PMC 2607343. PMID 18801719.
  21. ^ Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: the evolution of human cognition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. ISBN 978-0631188872.
  22. ^ Kenrick, D.T.; Li, N.P.; Butner, J. (2003). "Dynamical Evolutionary Psychology: Individual Decision Rules and Emergent Social Norms". Psychological Review. 110 (1): 3–28. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.110.1.3. PMID 12529056. S2CID 43306158.
  23. ^ Fog, Agner (2017). Warlike and Peaceful Societies: The Interaction of Genes and Culture. Open Book Publishers. doi:10.11647/OBP.0128. ISBN 978-1-78374-403-9.

Read other articles:

För efternamnet, se Malm (efternamn). För andra betydelser, se Malm (olika betydelser). Bandad Järnmalm från Ukraina Malm är något av de mineral, bergarter eller andra beståndsdelar av berggrunden (eller lösa jordlager) som kan användas för utvinning av metaller eller andra värdefulla ämnen. Endast de mineral eller bergarter som innehåller ett värdefullt ämne i tillräckligt hög koncentration och i lämplig form för ekonomiskt lönsam utvinning kallas malmer. Malm k...

 

 

Kongres Amerika Serikat ke-54Gedung Capitol (1906)Periode4 Maret 1895 – 4 Maret 1897Anggota90 senator357 anggota dewan4 delegasi tanpa suaraMayoritas SenatPartai Republik (pluralitas)Presiden SenatAdlai E. Stevenson (D)Mayoritas DPRPartai RepublikKetua DPRThomas B. Reed (R)Pres. Senat Pro TemporeWilliam P. Frye (R)Sesike-1: 2 Desember 1895 – 11 Juni 1896ke-2: 7 Desember 1896 – 3 Maret 1897ke-53 ←→ ke-55 Kongres Amerika Serikat ke-54 adalah sebuah pertemuan cabang legi...

 

 

GSF Explorer, kapal pengebor minyak. Kapal pengebor adalah kapal motor yang telah dilengkapi dengan peralatan untuk aktivitas pengeboran minyak atau gas alam. Jenis Berikut adalah beberapa jenis dari kapal pengebor, di antaranya yaitu: Chikyū Salah satu kapal bor paling terkenal di dunia adalah kapal pengeboran laut milik Jepang, yaitu Chikyū yang sebenarnya adalah kapal penelitian. Chikyū memiliki kemampuan luar biasa untuk mengebor hingga ke kedalaman 7,0 kilometer (23.000 kaki).[1&#...

قرية غالواي الإحداثيات 43°01′00″N 74°02′02″W / 43.0167°N 74.0339°W / 43.0167; -74.0339   [1] تقسيم إداري  البلد الولايات المتحدة[2]  التقسيم الأعلى مقاطعة ساراتوغا  خصائص جغرافية  المساحة 0.662979 كيلومتر مربع (1 أبريل 2010)  ارتفاع 253 متر  عدد السكان  عدد السكان ...

 

 

Bandar Udara Fukushima福島空港福島空港Fukushima KūkōTerminal penumpang Bandar Udara Fukushima (2006)IATA: FKSICAO: RJSFInformasiJenisPublikPengelolaPemerintah Prefektur FukushimaMelayaniSukagawa, Fukushima, JepangKetinggian dpl mdplSitus webfks-ab.co.jpPetaRJSFLokasi di JepangLandasan pacu Arah Panjang Permukaan m kaki 01/19 2.500 8.202 Aspal beton Bandar Udara Fukushima (福島空港code: ja is deprecated , Fukushima Kūkō) (IATA: FKS, ICAO: RJSF)[1] merupak...

 

 

Синелобый амазон Научная классификация Домен:ЭукариотыЦарство:ЖивотныеПодцарство:ЭуметазоиБез ранга:Двусторонне-симметричныеБез ранга:ВторичноротыеТип:ХордовыеПодтип:ПозвоночныеИнфратип:ЧелюстноротыеНадкласс:ЧетвероногиеКлада:АмниотыКлада:ЗавропсидыКласс:Пт�...

Artikel ini perlu diwikifikasi agar memenuhi standar kualitas Wikipedia. Anda dapat memberikan bantuan berupa penambahan pranala dalam, atau dengan merapikan tata letak dari artikel ini. Untuk keterangan lebih lanjut, klik [tampil] di bagian kanan. Mengganti markah HTML dengan markah wiki bila dimungkinkan. Tambahkan pranala wiki. Bila dirasa perlu, buatlah pautan ke artikel wiki lainnya dengan cara menambahkan [[ dan ]] pada kata yang bersangkutan (lihat WP:LINK untuk keterangan lebih lanjut...

 

 

У этого термина существуют и другие значения, см. Западный округ. Западный внутригородской округ город Краснодар Дата основания 1936 год Дата упразднения 1994 Прежние имена Кагановичский, Ленинский районы Микрорайоны Дубинка, Черёмушки, Покровка Площадь 22[1]  км² Насе...

 

 

此條目可参照英語維基百科相應條目来扩充。 (2021年5月6日)若您熟悉来源语言和主题,请协助参考外语维基百科扩充条目。请勿直接提交机械翻译,也不要翻译不可靠、低品质内容。依版权协议,译文需在编辑摘要注明来源,或于讨论页顶部标记{{Translated page}}标签。 约翰斯顿环礁Kalama Atoll 美國本土外小島嶼 Johnston Atoll 旗幟颂歌:《星條旗》The Star-Spangled Banner約翰斯頓環礁�...

1969 Mafia killing in Palermo, Sicily Viale Lazio massacreThe body of Michele Cavataio after the hit in Viale Lazio.LocationOffice in the Viale Lazio in Palermo, SicilyDate10 December 1969 7:30 p.m. (Central European Time)TargetMichele Cavataio, Mafia boss of the Acquasanta quarter of Palermo.Attack typeMassacreDeathsFive killedPerpetratorsMafia hit squad composed of Bernardo Provenzano, Calogero Bagarella, Emanuele D’Agostino, Damiano Caruso (mafioso), Gaetano Grado and Salvatore Riin...

 

 

Частина серії проФілософіяLeft to right: Plato, Kant, Nietzsche, Buddha, Confucius, AverroesПлатонКантНіцшеБуддаКонфуційАверроес Філософи Епістемологи Естетики Етики Логіки Метафізики Соціально-політичні філософи Традиції Аналітична Арістотелівська Африканська Близькосхідна іранська Буддій�...

 

 

Robert B. Sherman dan Richard M. Sherman di London Palladium pada 2002 saat penampilan perdana Chitty Chitty Bang Bang: The Stage Musical. Sherman Bersaudara adalah duo penulis lagu Amerika Serikat yang mengkhususkan diri dalam film musikal, yang terdiri dari Robert B. Sherman (19 Desember 1925 – 6 Maret 2012)[1] dan Richard M. Sherman (kelahiran 12 Juni 1928). Sherman Bersaudara menulis lagu film musikal yang paling banyak ketimbang tim penulis lagu lainnya dalam sejarah perfilman....

Unincorporated community in the state of Alaska, United States Unincorporated community in Alaska, United StatesCrow VillageUnincorporated communityCrow VillageLocation within the state of AlaskaCoordinates: 61°34′15″N 159°47′00″W / 61.57083°N 159.78333°W / 61.57083; -159.78333CountryUnited StatesStateAlaskaCensus areaBethelGovernment • State senatorLyman Hoffman (D) • State rep.Zach Fansler (D)Time zoneUTC-9 (Alaska (AKST))...

 

 

Species of bird Curlew sandpiper Non-breeding plumage Breeding plumage Conservation status Near Threatened  (IUCN 3.1)[1] Scientific classification Domain: Eukaryota Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata Class: Aves Order: Charadriiformes Family: Scolopacidae Genus: Calidris Species: C. ferruginea Binomial name Calidris ferruginea(Pontoppidan, 1763) Synonyms Erolia ferruginea Vieillot, 1816 The curlew sandpiper (Calidris ferruginea) is a small wader that breeds on the tund...

 

 

Red supergiant star in the constellation Carina CK Carinae An I band (near infrared) light curve for CK Carinae, plotted from ASAS data[1] Observation dataEpoch J2000      Equinox J2000 Constellation Carina Right ascension 10h 24m 25.36s[2] Declination −60° 11′ 29.0″[2] Apparent magnitude (V) 7.2 - 8.5[3] Characteristics Spectral type M3.5 Iab[4] B−V color index +2.21[2]...

Parliamentary device in the Polish–Lithuanian Commonwealth Sejm session at the Royal Castle, Warsaw, 1622 The liberum veto (Latin for free veto[a]) was a parliamentary device in the Polish–Lithuanian Commonwealth. It was a form of unanimity voting rule that allowed any member of the Sejm (legislature) to force an immediate end to the current session and to nullify any legislation that had already been passed at the session by shouting either Sisto activitatem! (Latin: I stop the a...

 

 

Regional military unit size designation This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Military district – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (November 2007) (Learn how and when to remove this message) Military organization Typical units Typical numbers Typical commander fireteam 2–4 lance corpor...

 

 

Byzantine emperor in 641 Not to be confused with Constantine III (Western Roman emperor). Heraclius ConstantineEmperor of the RomansSolidus of Heraclius Constantine (right) with his father Heraclius (left)Byzantine emperorReign11 February – 25 May 641PredecessorHeracliusSuccessorHeraclonasCo-emperorHeraclonasBorn3 May 612[1]Died25 May 641[a] (aged 29)Chalcedon, Bithynia(now Kadıköy, Istanbul)SpouseGregoriaIssueConstans IITheodosiusManyanhNamesHeraclius Constantinus[4&...

1931 film The Night Without PauseDirected byAndrew MartonFranz WenzlerWritten byFranz Arnold (play)Ernst Bach (play)Bobby E. LüthgeWilly PragerProduced byJoe PasternakStarringSig ArnoCamilla HornMax AdalbertCinematographyKároly VassEdited byWolfgang BeckerMusic byOtto StranskyProductioncompaniesDeutsche Universal-FilmTobis FilmDistributed byDeutsche Universal-FilmRelease date 22 December 1931 (1931-12-22) Running time87 minutesCountryGermanyLanguageGerman The Night Without Pa...

 

 

Voce principale: Bologna. Questa voce o sezione sull'argomento torri non cita le fonti necessarie o quelle presenti sono insufficienti. Puoi migliorare questa voce aggiungendo citazioni da fonti attendibili secondo le linee guida sull'uso delle fonti. Panorama turrito di Bologna in una foto di Paolo Monti del 1965. Le torri di Bologna, strutture con funzione sia militare sia gentilizia di origine medievale, sono uno dei tratti più caratteristici della città. Delle torri presenti in an...