Human–animal communication

Human (Claudine André) and bonobo.

Human–animal communication is the communication observed between humans and other animals, ranging from non-verbal cues and vocalizations to the use of language.[1]

Some human–animal communication may be observed in casual circumstances, such as the interactions between pets and their owners, which can reflect a form of spoken, while not necessarily verbal dialogue. A dog being scolded is able to grasp the message by interpreting cues such as the owner's stance, tone of voice, and body language. This communication is two-way, as owners can learn to discern the subtle differences between barks or meows, and there is a clear difference between the bark of an angry dog defending its home and the happy bark of the same animal while playing. Communication (often nonverbal) is also significant in equestrian activities such as dressage.

One scientific study has found that 30 bird species and 29 mammal species share the same pattern of pitch and speed in basic messages. Therefore, humans and those 59 species can understand each other when they express "aggression, hostility, appeasement, approachability, submission and fear."[2][3]

Birds

Parrots are able to use words meaningfully in linguistic tasks.[4][failed verification] For example, a grey parrot named Alex learned one hundred words,[5] and after training used English words to answer questions about color, shapes, size and numbers correctly about 80% of the time.[6] He also wanted to try to go without training;[clarification needed] said where he wanted to be taken, such as his cage or the back of a chair; and protested when taken elsewhere or when hidden objects were not where he thought they were.[7] He asked what color he was,[7] which has been called the only question so far asked by a non-human animal.[8] Scientific American editor Madhusree Mukerjee described these abilities as creativity and reasoning comparable to nonhuman primates or cetaceans,[9] while expressing concern that extensive language use resulted in feather-plucking behavior, a possible sign of stress.

Most bird species have at least six calls which humans can learn to understand, for situations including danger, distress, hunger, and the presence of food.[10]

Pigeons can identify different artists.[11] Pigeons can learn to recognize up to 58 four-letter English words, with an average of 43, though they were not taught any meanings to associate with the words.[12]

Java sparrows chose music by sitting on a particular perch, which determined which music was played. Two birds preferred Bach and Vivaldi over Schoenberg or silence. The other two birds had varying preferences among Bach, Schoenberg, white noise and silence.[13]

The greater honeyguide has a specific call to alert humans that it can lead them to honey, and also responds to a specific human call requesting such a lead. By leading humans to honeybee hives, it can eat the discarded honeycomb wax after the honey is collected. The human call varies regionally, so the honeyguide's response is learned in each area, not instinctive.[14]

Crows identify and respond differently to different human faces and can be trained to understand and reply to verbal commands.[15][16]

Fictional portrayals of sentient talking parrots and similar birds are common in children's fiction,[17] such as the talking, loud-mouth parrot Iago of Disney's Aladdin.

Primates

Chimpanzees can make at least 32 sounds with distinct meanings for humans.[10]

Chimpanzees, gorillas and orangutans have used sign language, physical tokens, keyboards and touch screens to communicate with humans in numerous research studies. The research showed that they understood multiple signals and produced them to communicate with humans. There is a broad consensus among linguists that sign language experiments with great apes have not produced any evidence of true linguistic ability,[18][19] and high profile examples such as Koko have faced intense criticism, despite public perceptions of the experiments.

Baboons can learn to recognize an average of 139 four-letter English words (maximum of 308), though they were not taught any meanings to associate with the words.[20]

Primates have also been trained to use touch screens to tell a researcher their musical preferences. In Toronto, for hundreds of songs in random order, orangutans were given one 30-second segment of a song, and then chose between repeating that segment or 30 seconds of silence.[21] Different orangutans chose to replay from 8% to 48% of the segments, and all exhibited stress throughout the trials. There was no pattern of selections by genre, and the researchers did not look for other attributes that were shared by the orangutans' chosen segments. No comparison was available as to how many 30-second segments humans would repeat in the same situation. In another experiment, the orangutans did not distinguish between music played in its original order and music sliced into half-second intervals, which were played in random order. Chimpanzees can hear higher frequencies than humans. If orangutans can too, and if these overtones are present in the recordings, the overtones would affect their choices.[21]

Cetaceans

Lilly experiments

In the 1960s, neuroscientist John C. Lilly sponsored English lessons for one bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). The teacher, Margaret Howe Lovatt, lived with the dolphin for 2+12 months in a house on the shore of the Virgin Islands. The house was partially flooded and allowed them to be together for meals, play, language lessons, and sleep.[22][23][24][25] Lilly thought of this as a mother-child dyad, though the dolphin was five to six years old.[26] Lilly said that he had heard other dolphins repeating his own English words,[27] and believed that an intelligent animal would want to mimic the language of its captors, to communicate.[28][29] The experiment ended in the third month and did not restart, because Howe found the two-room lab and constant bumping from the dolphin too constricting.[30]

After several weeks, a concerted effort by the dolphin to imitate the instructor's speech was evident, and human-like sounds were apparent, and recorded. It was able to perform tasks such as retrieval of aurally indicated objects without fail. Later in the project the dolphin's ability to process linguistic syntax was made apparent, in that it could distinguish between commands such as "Bring the ball to the doll," and "Bring the doll to the ball."[citation needed] This ability not only demonstrates the bottlenose dolphin's grasp of basic grammar, but also implies the dolphins' own language might include syntactical rules. The correlation between length and 'syllables' (bursts of the dolphin's sound) with the instructor's speech also went from essentially zero at the beginning of the session to almost a perfect correlation by its completion. So that when the human spoke five or ten syllables, the dolphin also spoke five or ten 'syllables' or bursts of sound.[31]

Two experiments of this sort are explained in detail in Lilly's book, Mind of the Dolphin. The first experiment was more of a test run to check psychological and other strains on the human and cetacean participants. Its goal was to determine the extent of the need for other human contact, dry clothing, time alone, and so on. Despite tensions after several weeks, Howe Lovatt agreed to the 2+12 months isolated with the dolphin.

Herman experiments

Experiments by the research team of Louis Herman, a former collaborator and student of Lilly's, demonstrated that dolphins could understand human communication in whistles and respond with the same whistles.

A female bottlenose dolphin, Phoenix, understood at least 34 whistles.[32] Whistles created a system of two-way communication. By having separate whistles for object and action, Herman could reorder commands without fresh teaching (take hoop to ball). Successful communication was shown when Herman used new combinations, and the dolphin understood and did what he asked without further training 80–90% of the time.[33]

In 1980, Herman had taught six whistles to a female bottle-nose dolphin, Kea, to refer to three objects and three actions, and the dolphin followed his instructions. He wrote, "In addition to mouthing the three familiar training objects in the presence of the mouth name, Kea correctly mouthed on their first appearance a plastic water pipe, a wooden disc, and the experimenter's open hand. The same type of immediate response generalization occurred for touch and fetch."[34]

Richards, Wolz and Herman[35] (1984) trained a dolphin to make distinct whistles for objects, "so that, in effect, the dolphin gave unique vocal labels to those objects."

Herman's later publications do not discuss the whistle communication. Herman started getting US Navy funding in 1985,[36] so further expansion of the two-way whistle language would have been in the classified United States Navy Marine Mammal Program, a black project.

Herman also studied the crossmodal perceptual ability of dolphins. Dolphins typically perceive their environment through sound waves generated in the melon of their skulls, through a process known as echolocation (similar to that seen in bats, though the mechanism of production is different). The dolphin's eyesight however is also fairly good, even by human standards. Herman's research found that any object, even of complex and arbitrary shape, identified either by sight or sound by the dolphin, could later be correctly identified by the dolphin with the alternate sense modality with almost 100 per cent accuracy, in what is classically known in psychology and behaviorism as a match-to-sample test. The only errors noted were presumed to have been a misunderstanding of the task during the first few trials, and not an inability of the dolphin's perceptual apparatus. This capacity is strong evidence for abstract and conceptual thought in the dolphin's brain, wherein an idea of the object is stored and understood not merely by its sensory properties; such abstraction may be argued to be of the same kind as complex language, mathematics, and art, and implies a potentially very great intelligence and conceptual understanding within the brains of tursiops and possibly many other cetaceans. Accordingly, Lilly's interest later shifted to whale song and the possibility of high intelligence in the brains of large whales, and Louis Herman's research at the now misnomered Dolphin Institute in Honolulu, Hawaii, focuses exclusively on the Humpback whale.

Other researchers

  • Batteau[36] (1964, video) developed machines for the US Navy, which translated human voices to higher frequencies for dolphins to hear and translated dolphin voices to lower frequencies for humans to hear.[37][38] The work continued at least until 1967 when the Navy classified its dolphin research.[39] Batteau died, also in 1967, before he published results.
  • Soviet icebreaker (1985) experimented with different kinds of music to signal to 1,000 to 3,000 belugas that they could follow the ship to open water. They responded to classical music.[40][41][42]
  • Reiss and McCowan (1993) taught dolphins three whistles (ball, ring, rub), which the two dolphins produced, and even combined, when playing with the ball and/or ring, or getting a rub.[43]
  • Delfour and Marten (2005) gave dolphins a touchscreen to show they recognized a musical note[44]
  • Kuczaj (2006) used an underwater keyboard, which humans and dolphins can touch to signal an action.[45]
  • Amundin et al. (2008) had dolphins point narrow echolocation beams onto an array of hydrophones which acted like a touchscreen to communicate with the researchers[46] (video)
  • Reiss (2011) used an underwater keyboard which dolphins could press.[47] A dolphin defined a key as "I want a small fish".[48]
  • Herzing (2013) used an underwater keyboard in the open ocean which dolphins and humans could press to choose a plaything.[49]
  • Herzing (2014) created 3 whistles for "play objects (Sargassum... scarf, and rope)", and found that wild dolphins understand them, but has not found if dolphins produce the whistles.[50]

Historical

From Roman times to modern Brazil, dolphins have been known to drive fish toward fishermen waiting on shore, and signal to the fishermen when to throw their nets, even when water is too murky for the fishermen to see the arrival of the fish. The dolphins catch unnetted fish disoriented by the net.[51][52]

From about 1840 to 1920, orcas smacked the water off Twofold Bay in New South Wales to signal to human whalers that the orcas were herding large baleen whales nearby, so the humans would send boats to harpoon the whales, killing them faster and more assuredly than the orcas could. The orcas ate the tongues and lips, leaving the blubber and bones for the humans.[53][54]

Dogs

Origins of communication with canines

It has been widely theorized that human-animal communication began with the domestication of dogs.[55] Humans began communicating with wolves before the end of the late pleistocene,[56] and the two species eventually created a wide scale symbiotic relationship with one another. Modern biologists and anthropologists theorize that humans and wolves met near hunting grounds, and as the Homo sapiens diet began relying more and more on meat for development, they would often encounter and compete with wolves.[55]

Neolithic dog

Humans' relationship with wolves garnered a mutual benefit, obtaining food and protection.[56] Humans likely began attempting to cooperate with wolves through commands, which eventually led to a more familiar species of dogs that we know today. These commands were likely the first instances of obedience training upon canines, as dogs maintained a pack mentality that humans fit into as a pack member.[55]

Neolithic humans developed, seemingly unintentionally, a system of artificial selection with both livestock and animal companions,[57] ushering in a widespread sustenance based foundation of humans communicating with animals. New theories within academic discussion of scientific data refer to this as both prezygotic and postzygotic “strong” artificial selection.[57] Humans began controlling the offspring of livestock during the agricultural revolution through the mating of high yielding animals.[57]

Theories from anthropologists suggest that humans began differing relationships with canines during the neolithic age. This is possibly when humans began keeping dogs as pets, creating a new form of communication with domesticated animals, pet talk.

Dogs communicating to humans

In a Scientific American article from May 1884, John Lubbock described experiments teaching a dog to read text commands on cardboard cards.[58]

Bonnie Bergin trained dogs to go to specific text on the wall to ask clearly for "water, treat or pet me." Dogs were able to learn English or Japanese text. She says service dogs can learn to find EXIT signs, bathroom gender signs, and report what disease they smell in a urine sample by going to a sign on the wall naming that disease.[59]

Police dogs and private dogs can be trained to "alert" when they find certain scents, including drugs, explosives, mines, scent of a suspect, fire accelerants, and bed bugs. The alert can be a specific bark or position, and can be accepted as evidence in courts of law.[60]

Stanley Coren identifies 56 signals which untrained dogs make and people can understand, including ten barks, five growls, eight other vocalizations, 11 tail signals, five ear and eye positions, five mouth signals and 12 body positions.[61] Faragó et al. describe research that humans can accurately categorize barks from unseen dogs as aggressive, playful, or stressed, even if they do not own a dog.[62][63] This recognizability has led to machine learning algorithms to categorize barks,[64] and commercial products and apps such as BowLingual.

Humans communicating to dogs

Dogs can be trained to understand hundreds of spoken words, including Chaser (1,022 words),[65] Betsy (340 words),[66] Rico (200 words),[67] and others.[10][61] They can react appropriately when a human uses verbs and nouns in new combinations, such as "fetch ball" or "paw frisbee."[65]

Canine researcher Bonnie Bergin trained dogs to obey 20 written commands on flashcards, in Roman or Japanese characters, including 🚫 to keep them away from an area.[59]

Shepherds and others have developed detailed commands to tell herding dogs when to move, stop, collect or separate herd animals.[68][69]

Mutual communication

Claims of interspecies communication between dogs and humans, with the use of sound buttons, have prompted researchers at the University of California, at San Diego to begin an ongoing research effort (as of June 2021) into potential canine linguistic capabilities.[70]

Felines

Human-Feline communication is dated to at least 9500–10000 B.C. according to archeological evidence from the Neolithic village of Shillourokambos on the Mediterranean island of Cyprus.[71] Human and cat remains were found buried together along with ceremonial seashells, polished stones, and other decorative artifacts.[71] This burial between a human and feline companion suggests that the two species had begun building a relationship with one another. Feline companions began with the establishment of organized wide-scale agriculture, as humans needed a way to exterminate vermin which inhabited food stores.[72]

Evidence of the regular domestication of felines started around 5000 B.C. in Ancient Egypt with cats becoming a tool which humans kept near food surpluses as agriculture became more widespread and regulated. Cats are known to possess a commensal relationship with humans, and are treated as regular housepets. Modern felines often perform no real duties and are housetrained. Human owners communicate with these felines through pet talk, yet there is little to no evidence that felines can understand humans or are capable of consistent training, most cases are individual and replication can be very difficult.[72]

Other animal training

Humans teach animals specific responses for specific conditions or stimuli. Training may be for purposes such as companionship, detection, protection, research and entertainment. During training humans communicate their wishes with positive or negative reinforcement. After training is finished the human communicates by giving signals with words, whistles, gestures, body language, etc.[73]

APOPO has trained Southern giant pouched rats to communicate to humans the presence of land mines, by scratching the ground, and tuberculosis in medical samples. They identify 40% more cases of tuberculosis than clinics do, an extra 12,000 cases from 2007 to 2017. They have identified 100,000 mines from 2003 to 2017, certifying 2,200 hectares (5,400 acres) as mine-free. They are accurate enough that the human trainers run on the land after removing the mines which rats have identified.[74]

Rats (Wistar, Rattus norvegicus) have been taught to distinguish and respond differently to different human faces.[75]

Patricia McConnell found that handlers around the world, speaking 16 languages, working with camels, dogs, donkeys, horses and water buffalo, all use long sounds with a steady pitch to tell animals to go more slowly (whoa, euuuuuu), and they use short repeated sounds, often rising in pitch, to speed them up or bring them to the handler (Go, Go, Go, claps, clicks). Chimpanzees, dogs, gulls, horses, rats, roosters, sheep and sparrows all use similar short repeated sounds to tell others of the same species to come closer.[76]

Even fish, which lack a neocortex, have been taught to distinguish and respond differently to different human faces (archerfish[77]) or styles of music (goldfish[78] and koi[79]).

Molluscs, with totally different brain designs, have been taught to distinguish and respond to symbols (cuttlefish[80] and octopus[81]), and have been taught that food behind a clear barrier cannot be eaten (squid[82]).

A harbor seal, Hoover learned to speak several phrases in understandable English as a pup from his human foster parent and used these in appropriate circumstances during his later life at the New England Aquarium until he died in 1985.[83] Other talking animals have been studied, though they did not always use their phrases in meaningful contexts.

Animal communication as entertainment

Poster for Toby the Sapient pig

Though animal communication has always been a topic of public comment and attention, for a period in history it surpassed this and became sensational popular entertainment. From the late 18th century through the mid 19th century, a succession of "learned pigs" and various other animals were displayed to the public in for-profit performances, boasting the ability to communicate with their owners (often in more than one language), write, solve math problems, and the like. One poster, dated 1817, shows a group of "Java sparrows" who are advertised as knowing seven languages, including Chinese and Russian.

See also

Bibliography

  • Sebeok, Thomas. Essays in Zoosemiotics (1990) ISSN 0838-5858
  • Myers, Arthur. Communicating With Animals: The Spiritual Connection Between People and Animals (1997) ISBN 0-8092-3149-2
  • Boehner, Bruce Thomas. Parrot Culture: Our 2,500-Year-Long Fascination with the World's Most Talkative Bird (2004) ISBN 0-8122-3793-5
  • Summers, Patty. Talking With the Animals (1998) ISBN 1-57174-108-9
  • Jay, Ricky. Learned Pigs and Fireproof Women (1987) ISBN 0-446-38590-5
  • Gurney, Carol. The Language of Animals: 7 Steps to Communicating with Animals (2001) ISBN 0-440-50912-2
  • Grandin, Temple. Animals in Translation: Using the Mysteries of Autism to Decode Animal Behavior (2004) ISBN 0-7432-4769-8
  • Ruesch, Jurgen, and Weldon Kees. Nonverbal Communication : Notes on the Visual Perception of Human Relations. University of California Press, 1956
  • Hurn, Samantha. Humans and Other Animals : Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Human-Animal Interactions. Pluto Press, 2012. Accessed 24 Feb. 2022
  • Bradshaw, G. A. “You See Me, but Do You Hear Me? The Science and Sensibility of Trans-Species Dialogue.” Feminism & Psychology, vol. 20, no. 3, Aug. 2010, pp. 407–419. EBSCOhost, https://doi.org/10.1177/0959353510368285
  • Brown, Peter. “Talking Points.” Natural History, vol. 113, no. 9, Nov. 2004, p. 8. EBSCOhost, https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=a9h&AN=14772118&site=ehost-live
  • Perconti, Pietro. “Context-Dependence in Human and Animal Communication.” Foundations of Science, vol. 7, no. 3, Sept. 2002, pp. 341–62. EBSCOhost, https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019613210814

References

  1. ^ Shah, Sonia (20 September 2023). "The Animals Are Talking. What Does It Mean? - Language was long understood as a human-only affair. New research suggests that isn't so. + comment". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 21 September 2023. Retrieved 21 September 2023.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  2. ^ Morton, Eugene, and Donald Henry Owings (1998). Animal vocal communication : a new approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521324687. OCLC 37239014.
  3. ^ Friend, Tim (2004). Animal talk : breaking the codes of animal language. New York: Free Press. pp. 90. ISBN 978-0743201575. OCLC 53144532.
  4. ^ "This Bird Is No Airhead: Scientist". 9 February 2004. Archived from the original on 15 December 2007. Retrieved 28 August 2023.
  5. ^ Carey, Benedict (10 September 2007). "Alex, a Parrot Who Had a Way With Words, Dies". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 23 February 2018.
  6. ^ "Ask the Scientists: Irene Pepperberg Q&A". PBS. Archived from the original on 18 October 2007. Retrieved 11 September 2007.
  7. ^ a b Wise, Steven M. (2002). Drawing the Line. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books. pp. 101–102, 107. ISBN 978-0-7382-0340-9.
  8. ^ Jordania, Joseph (2006). Who Asked the First Question? The Origins of Human Choral Singing, Intelligence, Language and Speech. Tbilisi: Logos. ISBN 978-99940-31-81-8.
  9. ^ Mukerjee, Madhusree (1996). "An interview with Alex, the African grey parrot". Scientific American Blog Network. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0496-28.
  10. ^ a b c Scott, John Paul (1972). Animal behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 190, 197. ISBN 978-0226743363.
  11. ^ Watanabe, Shigeru (1 November 2001). "Van Gogh, Chagall and pigeons: picture discrimination in pigeons and humans". Animal Cognition. 4 (3–4): 147–151. doi:10.1007/s100710100112. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 24777504. S2CID 21344708.
  12. ^ Scarf, Damian; Boy, Karoline; Reinert, Anelisie Uber; Devine, Jack; Güntürkün, Onur; Colombo, Michael (4 October 2016). "Orthographic processing in pigeons (Columba livia)". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113 (40): 11272–11276. Bibcode:2016PNAS..11311272S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1607870113. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 5056114. PMID 27638211.
  13. ^ Watanabe, Shigeru; Nemoto, M (1 May 1998). "Reinforcing property of music in Java sparrows (Padda oryzivora)". Behavioural Processes. 43 (2): 211–218. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(98)00014-X. ISSN 0376-6357. PMID 24896007. S2CID 12648961.
  14. ^ Spottiswoode, Claire N.; Begg, Keith S.; Begg, Colleen M. (22 July 2016). "Reciprocal signaling in honeyguide-human mutualism" (PDF). Science. 353 (6297): 387–389. Bibcode:2016Sci...353..387S. doi:10.1126/science.aaf4885. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 27463674. S2CID 206648494.
  15. ^ "The Crow Paradox". NPR.org. 27 July 2009. Retrieved 28 December 2017.
  16. ^ Tarter, R. R. (2008). The Vocal Behavior of the American Crow, Corvus brachyrhynchos [master's thesis]. The Ohio State University.
  17. ^ Boehrer, Bruce Thomas (2004). Parrot culture: our 2,500-year-long fascination with the world's most talkative bird. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. ISBN 978-0812237931. OCLC 54446320.
  18. ^ Tomasello, Michael (12 October 2017). "What did we learn from the ape language studies?". Bonobos: 95–104. doi:10.1093/oso/9780198728511.003.0007. ISBN 978-0-19-872851-1.
  19. ^ Magazine, Smithsonian; Raffaele, Paul. "Speaking Bonobo". Smithsonian Magazine.
  20. ^ Grainger, Jonathan; Dufau, Stéphane; Montant, Marie; Ziegler, Johannes C.; Fagot, Joël (13 April 2012). "Orthographic Processing in Baboons (Papio papio)" (PDF). Science. 336 (6078): 245–248. Bibcode:2012Sci...336..245G. doi:10.1126/science.1218152. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 22499949. S2CID 16902074. Archived from the original (PDF) on 29 December 2017. Retrieved 29 December 2017.
  21. ^ a b Ritvo, Sarah E.; MacDonald, Suzanne E. (2 August 2016). "Music as enrichment for Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii)". Journal of Zoo and Aquarium Research. 4 (3): 156–163. doi:10.19227/jzar.v4i3.231. Retrieved 28 August 2023.
  22. ^ Lilly, John Cunningham (1967). The mind of the dolphin : a nonhuman intelligence (1st ed.). Garden City, New York: Doubleday. ISBN 978-0385025430. OCLC 851669.
  23. ^ Lilly, John Cunningham (1978). Communication between man and dolphin : the possibilities of talking with other species. New York: Crown Publishers. ISBN 978-0517530368. OCLC 4036849.
  24. ^ Lilly, John (1962). Man and Dolphin. Gollancz.
  25. ^ Lilly, John (1975). Lilly on Dolphins. Anchor Press.
  26. ^ Lilly on Dolphins. p. 177.
  27. ^ Lilly, John C. (1962). "Vocal Behavior of the Bottlenose Dolphin". Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society. 106 (6): 520–529. JSTOR 985258.
  28. ^ Mind of the Dolphin. p. 218.
  29. ^ Mind of the Dolphin. p. 196.
  30. ^ Mind of the Dolphin. pp. 299–300.
  31. ^ Mind of the Dolphin. pp. 288–295.
  32. ^
  33. ^ Herman, Louis M.; Richards, Douglas G.; Wolz, James P. (1984). "Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins". Cognition. 16 (2): 129–219. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(84)90003-9. PMID 6540652. S2CID 43237011.
  34. ^ Herman, Louis (1980). Cetacean Behavior. Wiley. pp. 413–418.
  35. ^ Richards, D. G.; Wolz, J. P.; Herman, L. M. (March 1984). "Vocal mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 98 (1): 10–28. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.98.1.10. ISSN 0735-7036. PMID 6705501.
  36. ^ a b LeVasseur, Kenneth. "Dolphin Mental Abilities Paper". whales.org.au. Retrieved 21 August 2017.
  37. ^ Einhorn, Richard N. (6 December 1967). "Special Report - Dolphins challenge the designer" (PDF). Electronic Design 25. Retrieved 30 November 2023.
  38. ^ Moshier, Stephen L. (13 December 1967). "Technical Manual MDT-5. Final Report, Appendix A, U. S. Navy Contract N00123-67-C-1103" (PDF). US Navy. Retrieved 30 November 2023.
  39. ^ Batteau, Dwight W., and Peter R. Markey (13 December 1967). "Man/Dolphin Communication. Final Report, U. S. Navy Contract N00123-67-C-1103" (PDF). US Navy. Retrieved 30 November 2023.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  40. ^ Remili, Anaïs (19 May 2024). "Russian ship played classical music to rescue entrapped belugas". Whale Scientists. Retrieved 22 May 2024.
  41. ^ Schmemann, Serge (12 March 1985). "RUSSIANS TELL SAGA OF WHALES RESCUED BY AN ICEBREAKER". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 22 May 2024.
  42. ^ "Soviet Ship Cracks Wall of Ice to Free Trapped Whales". Los Angeles Times. 22 February 1985. Retrieved 22 May 2024.
  43. ^ Reiss, Diana; McCowan, Brenda (1993). "Spontaneous Vocal Mimicry and Production by Bottlenose Dolphins: Tursiops truncatus: Evidence for Vocal Learning" (PDF). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107 (3): 301–312. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.107.3.301. PMID 8375147. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 August 2017. Retrieved 21 August 2017.
  44. ^ Delfour, Fabienne; Marten, Ken (2005). "Inter-modal learning task in bottlenosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): a preliminary study showed that social factors might influence learning strategies". Acta Ethologica. 8 (1): 57–64. doi:10.1007/s10211-005-0110-z. S2CID 43412127.
  45. ^ Kuczaj (2006). Problem Solving and Behavioral Flexibility, in: Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence edited by Edward A. Wasserman, Thomas R. Zentall. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780195167658. Retrieved 22 August 2017.
  46. ^ Amundin, Mats; Starkhammar, Josefin; Evander, Mikael; Almqvist, Monica; Lindström, Kjell; Persson, Hans W. (1 February 2008). "An echolocation visualization and interface system for dolphin research" (PDF). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 123 (2): 1188–1194. Bibcode:2008ASAJ..123.1188A. doi:10.1121/1.2828213. ISSN 0001-4966. PMID 18247918. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 August 2017. Retrieved 22 August 2017.
  47. ^ Reiss, Diana (2011). "Dolphin in the Mirror website". Houghton Mifflin. Archived from the original on 15 January 2012. Retrieved 22 August 2017.
  48. ^ Reiss, Diana (2011). Dolphin in the Mirror book. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. ISBN 978-0547445724. Retrieved 22 August 2017.
  49. ^ Herzing, Denise (February 2013). "Could We Speak the Language of Dolphins?". Retrieved 22 August 2017.
  50. ^ Herzing, Denise (31 March 2014). "CHAT: Is It A Dolphin Translator Or An Interface?". wilddolphinproject.org. Retrieved 21 August 2017.
  51. ^ Pryor, Karen; Lindbergh, Jon (1 January 1990). "A Dolphin-Human Fishing Cooperative in Brazil". Marine Mammal Science. 6 (1): 77–82. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1990.tb00228.x. ISSN 1748-7692.
  52. ^ Zappes, Camilah Antunes; Andriolo, Artur; Simões-Lopes, Paulo César; Beneditto, Ana Paula Madeira Di (22 February 2011). "'Human-dolphin (Tursiops truncatus Montagu, 1821) cooperative fishery' and its influence on cast net fishing activities in Barra de Imbé/Tramandaí, Southern Brazil". Ocean & Coastal Management. 54 (5): 427–432. Bibcode:2011OCM....54..427Z. doi:10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2011.02.003.
  53. ^ Clode, Danielle (2002). Killers in Eden : the true story of killer whales and their remarkable partnership with the whalers of Twofold Bay. Crows Nest, N.S.W.: Allen & Unwin. ISBN 978-1865086521. OCLC 52250051.
  54. ^ "Australia's whaling industry and whales, Twofold Bay – unique teamwork with killer whales". www.australia.gov.au. Archived from the original on 7 September 2017. Retrieved 7 September 2017.
  55. ^ a b c Shipman, Pat (10 March 2015). The Invaders. Harvard University Press. doi:10.4159/9780674425385. ISBN 978-0-674-42538-5.
  56. ^ a b Botigué, Laura R.; Song, Shiya; Scheu, Amelie; Gopalan, Shyamalika; Pendleton, Amanda L.; Oetjens, Matthew; Taravella, Angela M.; Seregély, Timo; Zeeb-Lanz, Andrea; Arbogast, Rose-Marie; Bobo, Dean (18 July 2017). "Ancient European dog genomes reveal continuity since the Early Neolithic". Nature Communications. 8 (1): 16082. Bibcode:2017NatCo...816082B. doi:10.1038/ncomms16082. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 5520058. PMID 28719574. S2CID 297141.
  57. ^ a b c Driscoll, Carlos A.; Macdonald, David W.; O'Brien, Stephen J. (16 June 2009). "From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (supplement_1): 9971–9978. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.9971D. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901586106. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2702791. PMID 19528637.
  58. ^ Scientific American. Munn & Company. 24 May 1884. p. 325.
  59. ^ a b Bergin, Bonnie (2006). Teach your dog to read : a unique step-by-step program to expand your dog's mind and strengthen the bond between you (1st ed.). New York: Broadway Books. pp. 2, 3, 8, 139, 182–5. ISBN 978-0767922456. OCLC 61211431.
  60. ^ Ensminger, John (2012). Police and military dogs : criminal detection, forensic evidence, and judicial admissibility (PDF). Boca Raton: CRC Press. ISBN 978-1-4398-7240-6. OCLC 756484228.
  61. ^ a b Coren, Stanley (1994). The intelligence of dogs : canine consciousness and capabilities. New York: Free Press. pp. 93–97, 100–109. ISBN 978-0029066836. OCLC 29703107.
  62. ^ Faragó, Tamás; Townsend, Simon; Range, Friederike (18 November 2013). "The information content of wolf (and dog) social communication". In Guenther, Witzany (ed.). Biocommunication of animals. Dordrecht: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-7414-8. ISBN 9789400774131. OCLC 865545997.
  63. ^ Meegahapola, L.; Constantinides, M.; Radivojevic, Z.; Li, H.; Quercia, D.; Eggleston, M. S. (2023). "Quantified Canine: Inferring Dog Personality from Wearables". Proceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. pp. 1–19. arXiv:2301.06964. doi:10.1145/3544548.3581088. ISBN 9781450394215. S2CID 255942162.
  64. ^ Molnár, Csaba; Kaplan, Frédéric; Roy, Pierre; Pachet, François; Pongrácz, Péter; Dóka, Antal; Miklósi, Ádám (1 July 2008). "Classification of dog barks: a machine learning approach". Animal Cognition. 11 (3): 389–400. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.140.9111. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0129-9. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 18197442. S2CID 8577163.
  65. ^ a b Pilley, John W.; Reid, Alliston K. (2011). "Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents" (PDF). Behavioural Processes. 86 (2): 184–195. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.11.007. PMID 21145379. S2CID 18753940. Archived from the original (PDF) on 31 August 2019. Retrieved 1 December 2017.
  66. ^ Grassmann, Susanne; Kaminski, Juliane; Tomasello, Michael (1 July 2012). "How two word-trained dogs integrate pointing and naming". Animal Cognition. 15 (4): 657–665. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0494-x. ISSN 1435-9448. PMC 3377900. PMID 22526689.
  67. ^ Kaminski, Juliane; Josep Call; Julia Fischer (11 June 2004). "Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for "Fast Mapping"" (PDF). Science. 304 (5677): 1682–3. Bibcode:2004Sci...304.1682K. doi:10.1126/science.1097859. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 15192233. S2CID 31901162. Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 January 2013. Retrieved 2 January 2013.
  68. ^ Holland, Vergil (1994). Herding dogs : progressive training. New York: Howell Book House. ISBN 978-0876056448. OCLC 29844283.
  69. ^ Hartnagle-Taylor, Jeanne Joy (2010). Stockdog savvy. Crawford, CO: Alpine Publications. ISBN 978-1577791065. OCLC 456420606.
  70. ^ Sanchez, Kait (11 November 2020). "How Bunny the dog is pushing scientists' buttons". The Verge. Retrieved 2 June 2021.
  71. ^ a b "Oldest Known Pet Cat? 9,500-Year-Old Burial Found on Cyprus". Animals. 8 April 2004. Archived from the original on 23 February 2021. Retrieved 5 May 2022.
  72. ^ a b Driscoll, Carlos A.; Clutton-Brock, Juliet; Kitchener, Andrew C.; O'Brien, Stephen J. (June 2009). "The Taming of the Cat". Scientific American. 300 (6): 68–75. Bibcode:2009SciAm.300f..68D. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0609-68 (inactive 2 November 2024). ISSN 0036-8733. S2CID 73704872.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of November 2024 (link)[permanent dead link]
  73. ^ Ramirez, Ken (1999). Animal training : successful animal management through positive reinforcement. Chicago, IL: Shedd Aquarium. ISBN 978-0961107499. OCLC 45076489.
  74. ^ Lyons, Kate (26 December 2017). "The giant rats that love avocado – and can diagnose deadly TB". The Guardian. Retrieved 26 December 2017.
  75. ^ Otsuka, Y.; Yanagi, J.; Watanabe, S. (1 February 2009). "Discriminative and reinforcing stimulus properties of music for rats". Behavioural Processes. 80 (2): 121–127. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2008.10.009. ISSN 0376-6357. PMID 19022358. S2CID 24708690.
  76. ^ McConnell, Patricia (2002). The other end of the leash : why we do what we do around dogs (1st ed.). New York: Ballantine Books. pp. 57–58, 63. ISBN 978-0345446794. OCLC 49870666.
  77. ^ Newport, Cait; Wallis, Guy; Reshitnyk, Yarema; Siebeck, Ulrike E. (7 June 2016). "Discrimination of human faces by archerfish (Toxotes chatareus)". Scientific Reports. 6 (1): 27523. Bibcode:2016NatSR...627523N. doi:10.1038/srep27523. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 4895153. PMID 27272551.
  78. ^ Shinozuka, Kazutaka; Ono, Haruka; Watanabe, Shigeru (2013). "Reinforcing and discriminative stimulus properties of music in goldfish". Behavioural Processes. 99: 26–33. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2013.06.009. PMID 23796771. S2CID 439990.
  79. ^ Chase, Ava R. (1 November 2001). "Music discriminations by carp (Cyprinus carpio)". Animal Learning & Behavior. 29 (4): 336–353. doi:10.3758/bf03192900. ISSN 0090-4996.
  80. ^ Hough, Alexander; Boal, Jean (1 January 2014). "Automation of Discrimination Training for Cuttlefish (Mollusca: Cephalopoda)". Keystone Journal of Undergraduate Research. 2: 15–21 – via Shippensburg University.
  81. ^ Bublitz, Alexander; Weinhold, Severine R.; Strobel, Sophia; Dehnhardt, Guido; Hanke, Frederike D. (2017). "Reconsideration of Serial Visual Reversal Learning in Octopus (Octopus vulgaris) from a Methodological Perspective". Frontiers in Physiology. 8: 54. doi:10.3389/fphys.2017.00054. ISSN 1664-042X. PMC 5294351. PMID 28223940.
  82. ^ Zepeda, Emily A.; Veline, Robert J.; Crook, Robyn J. (1 June 2017). "Rapid Associative Learning and Stable Long-Term Memory in the Squid Euprymna scolopes". The Biological Bulletin. 232 (3): 212–218. doi:10.1086/693461. ISSN 0006-3185. PMID 28898600. S2CID 19337578.
  83. ^ Hiss, Anthony (3 January 1983). "Talk of the Town HOOVER". The New Yorker. Retrieved 20 December 2017.