Ego depletion is the idea that self-control or willpower draws upon conscious mental resources that can be taxed to exhaustion when in constant use with no reprieve (with the word "ego" used in the psychoanalytic sense rather than the colloquial sense).[1] When the energy for mental activity is low, self-control is typically impaired, which would be considered a state of ego depletion. In particular, experiencing a state of ego depletion impairs the ability to control oneself later on. A depleting task requiring self-control can have a hindering effect on a subsequent self-control task, even if the tasks are seemingly unrelated. Self-control plays a valuable role in the functioning of the self on both individualistic and interpersonal levels. Ego depletion is therefore a critical topic in experimental psychology, specifically social psychology, because it is a mechanism that contributes to the understanding of the processes of human self-control. There have both been studies to support[2] and to question[3] the validity of ego-depletion as a theory.[4]
Some meta-analyses and studies have questioned the size and existence of the ego depletion effect.[3] The ultimate validity and conclusions of those later studies are not universally agreed upon.[5] Martin Hagger and Nikos Chatzisrantis, whose 2010 meta-analysis seemed to support the existence of the ego depletion effect,[5] subsequently performed a pre-registered 23-lab replication study that did not find an ego-depletion effect.[6]
Early experimental evidence
American social psychologist Roy Baumeister and his colleagues proposed a model that described self-control like a muscle, which can become both strengthened and fatigued. The researches proposed that initial use of the “muscle” of self-control could cause a decrease in strength, or ego depletion, for subsequent tasks. Later experimental findings showed support for this muscle model of self-control and ego depletion.[2]
A key experiment by Baumeister, Ellen Bratslavsky, Mark Muraven, and Dianne Tice in 1998, demonstrated some of the first evidence that ego depletion had effects in diverse contexts or situations.[1] They showed that people who initially resisted the temptation of chocolates were subsequently less able to persist on a difficult and frustrating puzzle task. They attributed this effect to ego depletion, which resulted from the prior resisting of a tempting treat. Additionally, it was demonstrated that when people voluntarily gave a speech that included beliefs contrary to their own, they were also less able to persist on the difficult puzzle, indicating a state of ego depletion. This effect was not nearly as strong when individuals were not given a choice and were "forced" to write a counter-attitudinal speech. Thus, they proposed that both the act of choice and counter-attitudinal behaviors draw upon the same pool of limited resources. While giving a counter-attitudinal speech is expected to produce ego depletion, introducing the element of choice further increases the level of experienced depletion. These findings demonstrated the effects of ego depletion in differential situations and emphasized that ego depletion may not be context-specific. This experiment was critical in that the researchers synthesized ideas proposed by prior studies that had suggested evidence for a strength model of willpower. With this study, Baumeister and his colleagues therefore provided the first direct experimental evidence of ego depletion, and initiated research interest on the subject.
Physiological causes
The role of glucose as a specific form of energy needed for self-control has been explored by researchers. Glucose, a sugar found in many foods, is the primary fuel for the body and the brain. Multiple experiments have connected self-control depletion to reduced blood glucose, and suggested that self-control performance could be replenished by consuming glucose.[7] Some of the findings were later questioned.[8] However, several recent experiments have found that resource depletion effects can be reversed by simply tasting (but not swallowing or consuming) sweet beverages,[9][10][11] which can have rewarding properties. Others have suggested that the taste of sugar (but not artificial sweetener) has psycho-physiological signaling effects.[12]
A 2007 experiment by Segertrom and Nes found HRV (heart rate variability) to be a marker for ego depletion as well as an index for self-control power before the task.[13]
Neural activity associated with self-control failure has recently been examined using neurophysiological techniques. According to cognitive and neuroscientific models of mental control, a "conflict-monitoring/error-detection system" identifies discrepancies between intended goals and actual behaviors.[14]Error-related negativity (ERN) signals are a waveform of event-related potentials, which appear to be generated in the anterior cingulate cortex when individuals commit errors in various psychological tasks.[15] Using electroencephalography (EEG) recordings, Inzlicht and Gutsell found that individuals who had undergone an emotion-suppression task displayed weaker ERN signals compared to individuals who had not undergone emotion-suppression tasks.[14] These findings demonstrate preliminary evidence that depletion experienced after exerting self-control can weaken neural mechanisms responsible for conflict monitoring.
The majority of ego depletion studies have been carried out on university students, which raises concerns about how generalizable the results really are. The effects of age are unknown, but maybe younger people are more susceptible to the effects of ego depletion, given that the areas of the brain involved in self-control continue to develop until the mid 20s. For example, a recent study found that people over the age of 40 did not become ego depleted following a typical depletion manipulation, whereas younger university students did.[16]
Manifestations
Guilt and prosocial behavior
Ego depletion has also been implicated in guilt and prosocial behavior. The feeling of guilt, while unpleasant, is necessary to facilitate adaptive human interactions.[17] The experience of guilt is dependent on one's ability to reflect on past actions and behaviors. Ego depletion has been shown to hinder the ability to engage in such reflection, thereby making it difficult to experience guilt.[18] Since guilt typically leads to prosocial behavior, ego depletion will therefore reduce the good deeds that often result from a guilty conscience. In the study by Xu and colleagues, some participants were required to suppress their emotions while watching a movie about butchering animals, which resulted in a depleted state.[18] Participants were then induced to feel guilty by playing a game in which an opponent player was blasted with loud, unpleasant noises when they made errors. At the end of the experiment, participants were given a chance to leave money for a subsequent participant and were also given the choice of making a charitable donation. These were the measures of pro-social behavior. The results of this study indicated that people who experienced ego depletion felt less guilty and donated less money than non-depleted people. This demonstrates that ego depletion has an indirect effect on prosocial behavior by decreasing one's ability to experience guilt.
Perceived levels of fatigue
An individual's perceived level of fatigue has been shown to influence their subsequent performance on a task requiring self-regulation, independent of their actual state of depletion.[19] This effect is known as illusory fatigue. This was shown in an experiment in which participants engaged in a task that was either depleting or non-depleting, which determined each individual's true state of depletion. Ultimately, when participants were led to believe their level of depletion was lower than their true state of depletion, they performed much worse on a difficult working memory task. This indicates that an increased perceived level of fatigue can hinder self-regulatory performance independent of the actual state of depletion.
Motivation and beliefs
Ego depletion has been shown to have some rather debilitating consequences, most notably self-regulation impairments. These effects can, however, be temporarily buffered by external motivations and beliefs in unlimited willpower. An example of such an external motivator was demonstrated by Boucher and Kofos in 2012, where depleted participants who were reminded of money performed better on a subsequent self-control task.[20]
An experiment by Carol Dweck and subsequent work by Roy Baumeister and Kathleen Vohs have shown that beliefs in unlimited self-control help mitigate ego depletion for a short while, but not for long. Participants that were led to believe that they would not get fatigued performed well on a second task but were fully depleted on a third task.[21]
Real-life implications
In a state of ego depletion, an individual's impaired ability to self-regulate can be implicated in a wide range of undesirable and maladaptive behaviors, such as acts of aggression. Knowledge and strategies to counteract ego depletion would therefore be highly beneficial in various real-life situations. [citation needed]
Dieting
An experiment performed by Kathleen Vohs and Todd Heatherton demonstrated how ego depletion is particularly relevant when considering chronic dieters compared to non-dieters.[22] Chronic dieters constantly work at resisting their cravings and limiting their food intake. Vohs and Heatherton showed that the task of regulating food intake could be undermined in the face of tempting snacks, especially when the individual was experiencing a state of ego depletion. Both dieters and non-dieters attempted to suppress their emotional responses while watching a movie. Afterwards, participants were required to consume ice cream in order to engage in a taste test. The major finding was that dieters who suppressed their emotional responses to the movie experienced more ego depletion than those who were not required to suppress their emotions. Additionally, those individuals subsequently ate much more ice cream in the taste-testing task. Non-dieters did not show the same self-regulatory failures as dieters in these tasks. Therefore, it seems that the act of dieting itself is a form of resource expenditure. Dieters spend so much energy trying to limit their food intake that these efforts are likely to be undermined when faced with overwhelming temptation.
Athletic performance
Research has found that competitive athletes’ mental determination can be hindered after completing a difficult cognitive task more than after completing an easy cognitive task.[23] This indicates that the hindering effects of ego depletion can be applied not only to subsequent performance on cognitive tasks, but on physical tasks, as well.
Consumer behavior
In the world of consumerism, individuals are faced with decisions and choices that require the use of valuable energy resources in order to make informed purchases while resisting the temptation of impulsive or unnecessary purchases. Consumers are constantly bombarded with a broad range of options. In order to make the best choice, one must compare the many different aspects of various products. The complexity of consumer decisions in itself can result in ego depletion.[24] This, in turn, could impact any subsequent decisions consumers must make. When consumers are depleted, they are more likely to become passive, and they make more impulsive decisions that may not fall in line with their true values.
Consumers are faced with choices of different price ranges and product qualities in the market. Having many options can make consumers feel overwhelmed, causing ego depletion.[25] Advertisements telling consumers how they deserve and must have a product can cause mental fatigue and frustration, leading people to give in to buying a product.[26] Fatigue and frustration can also stem from deals with specific requirements on ways to purchase a product, along with spending effort on deciding which store has the best deals or trying to get to the store. People will then be led to buy the higher-priced or cheapest product.[24]
Consumers who have low self-control are susceptible to be more invested in obtaining products of high status.[27] These same consumers are more likely to be more motivated, persistent, and willing to pay more for a product. Doing so will lead consumers to have a sense of empowerment; they will feel in control again, as though they were overcoming their ego-depleted states. They may also be led to purchase a brand that has a high status, under the assumption that the brand is better.
Relief
A recent experiment showed that inducing a positive mood can buffer the impairing effects of ego depletion on subsequent performance.[28] Positive mood was induced by getting individuals to watch comedy videos or by giving them a surprise gift. Positive mood seemed to allow people to recover faster from ego depletion and, furthermore, improved their ability to self-regulate. There is no claim that positive mood can provide a general benefit to people who had not previously engaged in self-regulatory tasks; rather, positive mood can restore depleted individuals’ capacity to self-regulate. Furthermore, this experimental work does not consider in depth the mechanisms by which performance is restored. It is not known whether positive mood counteracts ego depletion or whether positive mood merely motivates an individual to persist in a task, despite one's depleted state.
The ego-depletion effect itself (without mood intervention) has, however, been shown to be unrelated to mood changes, as shown in multiple ego-depletion experiments that either controlled for mood or saw no mood changes. Thus, positive affect is just a way to counteract ego depletion after a person is depleted.
Theoretical explanations
Conservation hypothesis
The conservation hypothesis is a partial explanation of ego depletion. It suggests that there are two sorts of depletion:
When one is completely depleted and unable to self-control.
When one is only partly depleted. Still, one reduces one's self-control efforts to avoid complete exhaustion.[29]
According to this view, when people feel depleted, there might still exist a reserve store of energy to be used in extreme, high-priority situations that could be encountered in the future. This can be adaptive to the extent that expending any more resources at a given time might render an individual fully depleted of one's resources in an unexpected situation requiring self-regulation or other self-monitoring behaviours. The existence of a spare reservoir of mental energy ultimately explains why various motivators can buffer the effects of mild or moderate ego depletion. In a state of low resources, an individual lacks motivation to exert any more energy, but if motivation is presented, there are still extra resources that can be used up. Thus, ego depletion could be conceptualized as a psychological constraint necessary to safeguard precious resources that might be needed in emergency situations in the future. Under mild depletion, people still have a small amount of energy left in their "tank", which they do not have access to under normal circumstances.[29]
Criticism
Questions and alternative explanations
Although self-control has traditionally been thought of as a limited resource that can be depleted, some researchers disagree with this model. While multiple studies provided support for the ego-depletion effect, there is currently no direct measure of ego depletion, and studies mainly observe it by measuring how long people persist at a second task after performing a self-control task (the depleting task).[1] Furthermore, researchers usually examine the average task performance rather than the longitudinal performance trajectory. Only a few studies are available where performance trajectories were modelled. In two studies there was no evidence that the ego-depletion group performed worse in the first trials of the second task.[30][31]
Many ego-depletion studies, however, have shown that mood is not relevant to the results. In fact, many of the earlier experiments have tested for the effects of mood and saw no effect of mood whatsoever. Furthermore, the study and measurement of ego depletion may be affected by the confounding effect of cognitive dissonance. Researchers have questioned whether subjects are truly experiencing ego depletion, or whether the individuals are merely experiencing cognitive dissonance in the psychological tasks.[1]
Process model
In contrast to the original most known model of self-control, Michael Inzlicht and Brandon J. Schmeichel propose an alternative model of depletion, which they refer to as the process model.[32] This process model holds that initial exertions of willpower lead an individual's motivation to shift away from control, and towards gratification. As a part of this process, one's attention shifts away from cues that signal the need for control, and towards cues that signal indulgence. Inzlicht and Schmeichel argue that the process model provides a starting point for understanding self-control and that more research examining these cognitive, motivational, and affective influences on self-control is needed. A 2020 pre-registered study (686 participants) by Inzlicht and colleagues provided some evidence for this model.[33] They fitted computational models of decision making to show that when depleted, the decision boundary parameter[34][35] was reduced, suggesting that people disengage and become less interested in exerting further effort. Furthermore, they showed that depletion did not impair inhibitory control.
Reproducibility controversy and conflicting meta analyses
Although up until the mid-2010s there was widespread confidence in the robustness of the ego depletion effect, a substantial body of research has since cast doubt on the replicability of the effect.
A 2010 meta analysis of 198 independent tests found the effect significant with a moderate effect size (d = .6). Even after accounting for possible unpublished failed studies, the analysis concluded that it is extremely unlikely that the effect doesn't exist.[36] In 2015, a meta analysis of over 100 studies by Carter and McCullough argued that the 2010 meta-analysis failed to take publication bias into account. They showed statistical evidence for publication bias. When they statistically controlled for publication bias, the effect size estimate was small (d = .2) and not significantly different from zero.[37][38]Michael Inzlicht and colleagues praised Carter's meta analysis, but argued that bias-correction techniques are not precise enough to give a precise control size estimate.[5][39] In response, Cunningham and Baumeister argued that Carter and McCullough analysis contained errors in its data collection and in the various analyses used.[5]
Ulrich Schimmack (2016) conducted a meta-analysis of published studies and found that most studies could produce significant results only with the help of random sampling error. Based on the low power of studies, one would expect a large number of non-significant results, but these results are missing from published articles. This finding supports Carter and McCullough's meta-analysis that showed publication bias with a different statistical method. Schimmack's replicability report also identified a small set of studies with adequate power that provided evidence for ego-depletion. These studies are the most promising studies for a replication project to examine whether ego-depletion effects can be replicated consistently across several independent laboratories.[40]
In 2016, a major multi-lab replication study (2141 participants) carried out at two dozen labs across the world using a single protocol failed to find any evidence for ego depletion.[3][41] In response, Baumeister and Vohs argued that Baumeister's original protocol was rejected by the project coordinators, and after discussion was stalled, he only reluctantly agreed to a task that differed to some degree from the original 1998 studies.[42] However, a subsequent, separate multi-lab replication project, led by Kathleen Vohs and involving 36 labs testing 3531 participants,[43] also failed to find an ego-depletion effect (d = 0.06; an order of magnitude smaller than the effect size estimate in the original Hagger meta-analysis). Replication difficulties have also emerged for 5 additional protocols (operationalizations) of the basic ego depletion effect.[44]
^ abBaumeister, Roy F. (2002). "Ego Depletion and Self-Control Failure: An Energy Model of the Self's Executive Function". Self and Identity. 1 (2): 129–136. doi:10.1080/152988602317319302. S2CID12823588.
^Friese, Malte; Loschelder, David D.; Gieseler, Karolin; Frankenbach, Julius; Inzlicht, Michael (29 March 2018). "Is Ego Depletion Real? An Analysis of Arguments". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 23 (2): 107–131. doi:10.1177/1088868318762183. PMID29591537. S2CID4471882.
^Dorris, D. C.; Power, D. A.; Kenefick, E. (2011). "Investigating the effects of ego depletion on physical exercise routines of athletes". Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 13 (2): 118–125. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.10.004.
^ abBaumeister, R. F.; Sparks, E. A.; Stillman, T. F.; Vohs, K. D. (2008). "Free will in consumer behavior: Self-control, ego depletion, and choice". Journal of Consumer Psychology. 18: 4–13. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2007.10.002.
^Hofmann, W.; Strack, F.; Deutsch, R. (2008). "Free to buy? Explaining self-control and impulse in consumer behavior". Journal of Consumer Psychology. 18 (1): 22–26. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2007.10.005.
^Schmeichel, B.J.; Vohs, K.D.; Baumeister, R.F. (2003). "Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 85 (1): 33–46. CiteSeerX10.1.1.186.4312. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.33. PMID12872883. S2CID10100150.
^Cutright, Keisha M.; Samper, Adriana (2014). "Doing It the Hard Way: How Low Control Drives Preferences for High-Effort Products and Services". Journal of Consumer Research. 41 (3): 730–745. doi:10.1086/677314. hdl:2286/R.I.27265.
^"Supplemental Material for A Series of Meta-Analytic Tests of the Depletion Effect: Self-Control Does Not Seem to Rely on a Limited Resource". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2015. doi:10.1037/xge0000083.supp.
Hagger, Martin S.; Wood, Chantelle; Stiff, Chris; Chatzisarantis, Nikos L. D. (2010). "Ego Depletion and the Strength Model of Self-Control: A Meta-Analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 136 (4): 495–525. doi:10.1037/a0019486. PMID20565167. S2CID22705578.
Inzlicht, M.; Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). "What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (5): 450–463. doi:10.1177/1745691612454134. PMID26168503. S2CID3899310.
Daniel Kahneman (October 25, 2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. Macmillan. ISBN978-1-4299-6935-2
Muraven, M.; Baumeister, R. F. (2000). "Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (2): 247–259. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247. PMID10748642. S2CID10962693.
Lukisan Resi Anggira. Anggira (Dewanagari: अङ्गिरा atau अंगिरा; ,IAST: Aṅgirā, अङ्गिरा atau अंगिरा) adalah nama seorang resi dalam agama Hindu. Dia disebutkan dalam kitab Regweda sebagai guru pengetahuan suci, penghubung antara para dewa dan manusia, dan disebutkan dalam sloka lainnya sebagai Agni-dewa (dewa api) yang pertama.[1][2] Dalam sejumlah pustaka suci, ia dianggap sebagai salah satu tujuh resi yang agung...
Artikel ini sebatang kara, artinya tidak ada artikel lain yang memiliki pranala balik ke halaman ini.Bantulah menambah pranala ke artikel ini dari artikel yang berhubungan atau coba peralatan pencari pranala.Tag ini diberikan pada Desember 2023. Ini adalah daftar maskapai penerbangan yang saat ini beroperasi di Liberia. Maskapai penerbangan IATA ICAO Tanda panggil Mulaiberoperasi Catatan Liberia Airways LBA LIBERIA AIRWAYS Satgur Air Transport 2S TGR SATGURAIR Lihat pula Daftar maskapai pener...
Batas Armenia yang ditarik oleh Wilson Batas Armenia yang diusulkan oleh Woodrow Wilson (atau dalam bahasa Inggris disebut Wilsonian Armenia) adalah batas Republik Armenia Pertama yang diusulkan oleh Departemen Luar Negeri Amerika Serikat di bawah pemerintahan Presiden Woodrow Wilson dan kemudian ditetapkan oleh Perjanjian Sèvres.[1] Perjanjian Sèvres adalah perjanjian perdamaian yang dirumuskan dan ditandatangani oleh Sekutu dan Kesultanan Utsmaniyah pada Agustus 1920. Perjanjian i...
←→Июль Пн Вт Ср Чт Пт Сб Вс 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 2024 год Содержание 1 Праздники и памятные дни 1.1 Национальные 1.2 Региональные 1.3 Религиозные 1.4 Именины 2 События 2.1 До XIX века 2.2 XIX век 2.3 XX век 2.4 XXI век 3 Родились 3.1 До XIX века 3.2 XIX век 3.3 XX ве...
Japanese football club This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Sagan Tosu – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (August 2014) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Football clubSagan Tosu サガン鳥栖Full nameSagan Tosu Football ClubNickname(s)SaganFounded1997; 27...
Museum Hakka Indonesia (Yin-nì Hak-kâ Pok-vu̍t-kón)印尼客家博物館Lokasi Jakarta Timur, Jakarta, IndonesiaSitus webmuseumhakkaindonesia.com Museum Hakka Indonesia (Hanzi: 印尼客家博物館, Inggris: Indonesian Hakka Museum) adalah museum yang didirikan oleh Perhimpunan Hakka Indonesia Sejahtera yang berisikan informasi mengenai perjalanan sejarah Hakka di Indonesia. Museum ini berlokasi di komplek Taman Budaya Tionghoa, Taman Mini Indonesia Indah, Jakarta Timur, Jakarta,...
1975 American horror film directed by James Glickenhaus Not to be confused with The Astrologer (1976 film). The AstrologerTheatrical release posterDirected byJames Glickenhaus (as Jim Glickenhaus)Written byJames Glickenhaus (uncredited)Based onThe Astrologer by John CameronProduced byMark BuntzmanStarringBob ByrdMark BuntzmanJames GlickenhausCinematographyFrancis A. RomeroEdited byVictor ZimetMusic byBrad FiedelDistributed byRepublic ArtsRelease dateDecember 1975Running time96 minutesCountryU...
Opera Tiochiu Orang Tiochiu atau Teochew (Romanisasi Tiochiu:Diô-jiǔ-nâng) adalah sebuah subgrup Min Selatan yang dominan disamping Orang Hokkian. Orang Tiochiu merupakan salah satu kelompok Tionghoa yang banyak merantau ke berbagai tempat keluar Tiongkok.[1] Orang Tiochiu juga disebut Orang Tio-soa (Diô-soàⁿ/ Mandarin: Chaoshan), kependekan dari dua daerah asal mereka Diô-jiǔ dan Sòaⁿ-thâu . Sejarah singkat Tempat asal mula orang Tiochiu adalah Provinsi Fujian.[...
District of London This article is about the district in London. For other uses, see Hammersmith (disambiguation). Human settlement in EnglandHammersmithLyric Theatre, HammersmithHammersmithLocation within Greater LondonPopulation95,996 (2020)[1]OS grid referenceTQ233786• Charing Cross4.3 mi (6.9 km) ENELondon boroughHammersmith & FulhamCeremonial countyGreater LondonRegionLondonCountryEnglandSovereign stateUnited KingdomPost tow...
Baseball or softball pitcher who throws the first pitch for their team in a game Chris Young warms up in the bullpen before a game at Wrigley Field minutes before the 12:05 start (see scoreboard clock). In baseball (hardball or softball), a starting pitcher or starter is the first pitcher in the game for each team. A pitcher is credited with a game started if they throw the first pitch to the opponent's first batter of a game. Starting pitchers are expected to pitch for a significant portion ...
UAAP Season 71Host school University of the PhilippinesTaglineFilipino Leadership Through Sports ExcellenceOverallSeniors University of Santo TomasJuniors University of Santo TomasSeniors' championsSport Men WomenBasketball Ateneo FEUVolleyball UST La SalleChess FEU La SalleTable tennis UST FEUTaekwondo UST USTJudo Ateneo UPSwimming UST AteneoBeach Volleyball UST FEUTrack and field ...
County in Florida, United States County in FloridaLeon CountyCountyLeon County Courthouse FlagSealLocation within the U.S. state of FloridaFlorida's location within the U.S.Coordinates: 30°28′N 84°17′W / 30.46°N 84.28°W / 30.46; -84.28Country United StatesState FloridaFoundedDecember 29, 1824Named forJuan Ponce de LeónSeatTallahasseeLargest cityTallahasseeArea • Total702 sq mi (1,820 km2) • Land667 sq ...
Transport system providing air transport for hire Route map of the world's scheduled commercial airline traffic, 2009Commercial aviation is the part of civil aviation that involves operating aircraft for remuneration or hire, as opposed to private aviation. Definition Commercial aviation is not a rigorously defined category. All commercial air transport and aerial work operations are regarded as commercial aviation, as well as some general aviation flights. An aircraft operation involving the...
American politician in New York City (b. 1950) For the Venezuelan Paralympic athlete, see Fernando Ferrer (athlete). This biography of a living person needs additional citations for verification. Please help by adding reliable sources. Contentious material about living persons that is unsourced or poorly sourced must be removed immediately from the article and its talk page, especially if potentially libelous.Find sources: Fernando Ferrer – news · newspapers · boo...
This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: University High School Irvine, California – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (January 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this message) Comprehensive high school in Irvine, California, United StatesUniversity High SchoolEntrance to University Hi...
Untuk Kegunaan lain, lihat Xeon. artikel ini perlu dirapikan agar memenuhi standar Wikipedia. Tidak ada alasan yang diberikan. Silakan kembangkan artikel ini semampu Anda. Merapikan artikel dapat dilakukan dengan wikifikasi atau membagi artikel ke paragraf-paragraf. Jika sudah dirapikan, silakan hapus templat ini. (Pelajari cara dan kapan saatnya untuk menghapus pesan templat ini) XeonMasa produksi1998-sekarangProdusenIntelKecepatan prosesor400 MHz -4.0 GHzKecepatan FSB100 MHz ...
Polish coat of arms DeszpotDetailsBattle cry-Alternative names-Earliest mentionunknownTownsnoneFamilies10 names altogether: Despot, Deszpot, Kostrzeń, Kostrzeński, Kostrzyński, Mładanowicz, Zenowicz, Zieniewicz, Zienowicz, Żyniew Deszpot is a Polish coat of arms. It was used by several szlachta families in the times of the Polish–Lithuanian Commonwealth. Notable bearers Notable bearers of this coat of arms include: See also Polish heraldry Heraldry Coat of arms Dynastic Genealogy vte C...
بطليموس كيراونوس (بالإغريقية: Πτολεμαῖος Κεραυνός) معلومات شخصية تاريخ الميلاد -318 تاريخ الوفاة -279 سبب الوفاة قطع الرأس الزوجة أرسينوي الثانية الأب بطليموس الأول إخوة وأخوات أرسينوي الثانية، وبطليموس الثاني عائلة بطالمة الحياة العملية المهن�...
Peta menunjukan lokasi Dilasag Data sensus penduduk di Dilasag Tahun Populasi Persentase 199512.825—200014.6762.94%200714.8330.15% Dilasag adalah munisipalitas yang terletak di provinsi Aurora, Filipina. Pada tahun 2007, munisipalitas ini memiliki populasi sebesar 14.833 jiwa atau 3.011 rumah tangga. Pembagian wilayah Dilasag terbagi menjadi 11 barangay, yaitu: Diagyan Dicabasan Dilaguidi Dimaseset Diniog Lawang Maligaya (Pob.) Manggitahan Masagana (Pob.) Ura Esperanza Pranala luar Dilasag ...