Figure–ground (perception)

The Rubin vase illusion, where it is ambiguous which part is the figure and which the ground
Shapes which can be read as a word once the viewer recognises them as being the isolated negative spaces of a word

Figure–ground organization is a type of perceptual grouping that is a vital necessity for recognizing objects through vision. In Gestalt psychology it is known as identifying a figure from the background. For example, black words on a printed paper are seen as the "figure", and the white sheet as the "background".[1]

Gestalt psychology

The Gestalt theory was founded in the 20th century in Austria and Germany as a reaction against the associationist and structural schools' atomistic orientation.[2] In 1912, the Gestalt school was formed by Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Köhler, and Kurt Koffka. The word "gestalt" is a German word translated to English as "pattern" or "configuration."[3] Gestalt concepts can also be referred to as "holism."[4] Gestalt Psychologists were attempting to humanize what was considered a sterile approach. Gestalt psychology establishes that the whole of anything is greater than its parts. The concepts explored by Wertheimer, Köhler, and Koffka in the 20th century established the foundation for the modern study of perception.[5]

"The Gestalt concept is that "not only movement, or process as such, but also the direction and distribution of process is determined dynamically by interaction." Sensory organization is not dependent upon isolated stimuli and local stimulation, but upon the relative properties of stimulation and the dynamical context."[6]

Wertheimer described holism as the "fundamental formula" of Gestalt psychology: "There are wholes, the behavior of which is not determined by that of their individual elements, but where the part-processes are themselves determined by the intrinsic nature of the whole."[7]

Examples

Man and crane, Mimbres culture pot, c. 1000 -1150 AD

The Rubin vase faces–vase drawing that Danish psychologist Edgar Rubin described [8][9] exemplifies one of the key aspects of figure–ground organization, edge-assignment and its effect on shape perception. In the faces–vase drawing, the perceived shape depends critically on the direction in which the border (edge) between the black and white regions is assigned. If the edges between the black and white regions are assigned inward, then the central white region is seen as a vase shape in front of a black background. No faces are perceived in this case. On the other hand, if the edges are assigned outward, then the two black profile faces are perceived on a white background, and no vase shape is perceived. The human visual system will settle on either of the interpretations of the Rubin vase and alternate between them, a phenomenon known as multistable perception. Functional brain imaging shows that, when people see the Rubin image as a face, there is activity in the temporal lobe, specifically in the face-selective region.[10][11]

An additional example is the "My Wife and My Mother-in-Law" illusion drawing. The image is famous for being reversible. "The viewer may either observe a young girl with her head turned to the right or an old woman with a large nose and protruding chin, depending on one's perspective."[12]

Development

Figure–ground perception precedes all other visual perceptual skills and is one of the first to develop in a young baby.[13] The development of perceptual organization develops as early as infancy in human beings. In regards to nature versus nurture, concepts such as "lightness" and "proximity" may develop as early as birth, but recognizing "form similarity" may not be functional until activated by particular experiences.[14]

Three- to four-month olds respond to differences in lightness rather than differences in form similarity. It is suggested that scaffolding (the development of new skills over time based on the building of other skills) is responsible for the development of perceptual organization. Environment plays a major role in the development of figure-ground perception.[15]

The development of figure–ground perception begins the day the baby can focus on an object. The faces of caregivers, parents, and familiar objects are the first to be focused on and understood. As babies develop, they learn to distinguish the objects they desire from their surroundings. Sitting up, crawling, and walking present ample opportunity to develop the skill during development. Between the ages of 2–4 the skill can be further cultivated by teaching the child to group or sort items.[16]

Perceptual process

The perceptual decision in which the brain decides which item is the figure and which are part of the ground in a visual scene can be based on many cues, all of which are of a probabilistic nature. For instance, size assists in distinguishing between the figure and the ground, as smaller regions are often (but not always) figures. Object shape can assist in distinguishing figure from ground because figures tend to be convex. Movement also helps; the figure may be moving against a static environment. Color is also a cue because the background tends to continue as one color behind potentially multiple foreground figures, whose colors may vary. Edge assignment also helps; if the edge belongs to the figure, it defines the shape while the background exists behind the shape. However, it is sometimes difficult to distinguish between the two because the edge that would separate figure from ground is part of neither, equally defining both the figure and the background.[17]

The LOC (lateral occipital cortex) is highly important for figure–ground perception. This region of the visual cortex (located lateral to the fusiform gyrus and extending anteriorly and ventrally) has consistently shown stronger activation in response to objects versus non-objects."[18][19]

Evidently, the process of distinguishing figure from ground (sometimes called figure–ground segmentation) is inherently probabilistic, and the best that the brain can do is to take all relevant cues into account to generate a probabilistic best-guess. In this light, Bayesian figure–ground segmentation models have been proposed to simulate the probabilistic inference by which the brain may distinguish figure from ground.[20][21]

Subjective factors can also influence figure–ground perception. For instance, if a viewer has the intention to perceive one of the two regions as the figure, it will likely alter their ability to analyze the two regions objectively. In addition, if a viewer's gaze is fixated on a particular region, the viewer is more likely to view the fixated region as the figure. Although subjective factors can alter the probability of seeing the figure on one particular side of an edge, they tend not to overpower compositional cues.[22]

Artistic applications

Figure-ground ambigram tessellation displaying the words Liar / Jail alternatively, using negative space.

Figure–ground organization is used to help artists and designers in composition of a 2D piece. Figure–ground reversal may be used as an intentional visual design technique in which an existing image's foreground and background colors are purposely swapped to create new images.

Non-visual

Figure–ground perception can be expanded from visual perception to include non-visual concepts such as melody/harmony, subject/background and positive/negative space.[citation needed] The concept of figure and ground fully depends on the observer and not on the item itself.[23]

In the typical sonic scenarios people encounter, auditory figure and ground signals often overlap in time as well as in frequency content. In these situations, auditory objects are established by integrating sound components both over time and frequency.[24] A 2011 study suggests that the auditory system possesses mechanisms that are sensitive to such cross-frequency and cross-time correlations. Results of this study demonstrated significant activations in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the superior temporal sulcus related to bottom-up, stimulus-driven figure–ground decomposition.[25]

In crowded rooms or parties, a person is able to zero in on the conversation they are having with one person (figure) while drowning out the background noise (ground). This can also be referred to as the "cocktail party effect."[26]

Figure–ground segregation in hearing is not automatic; rather, it requires attention[27] and draws on resources that are shared across vision and audition.[28]

Types of figure–ground problems

Type 2: The ground itself is the figure.

There are three types of figure–ground problems:

  1. The figure and the ground compete.
  2. The figure should be the ground and the ground should be the figure.
  3. The figure and ground create an optical illusion.[29]

See also

References

  1. ^ Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. "Chapter 4: Sensation and Perception, Vision II: Recognizing What We Perceive." Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 149-50. Print.
  2. ^ "Gestalt psychology | Definition, Founder, Principles, & Examples". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 2019-12-10.
  3. ^ "gestalt", The Free Dictionary, retrieved 2019-12-10
  4. ^ Wagemans, Johan; Feldman, Jacob; Gepshtein, Sergei; Kimchi, Ruth; Pomerantz, James R.; van der Helm, Peter A.; van Leeuwen, Cees (2012). "A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: II. Conceptual and theoretical foundations". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1218–1252. doi:10.1037/a0029334. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 3728284. PMID 22845750.
  5. ^ "Information TheoryReprinted with permission from Encyclopædia Britannica, 14th edition, 1968 by Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc.", Claude E. Shannon, IEEE, 2009, doi:10.1109/9780470544242.ch14, ISBN 978-0-470-54424-2
  6. ^ "PsycNET". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2019-12-10.
  7. ^ Wagemans, Johan; Feldman, Jacob; Gepshtein, Sergei; Kimchi, Ruth; Pomerantz, James R.; van der Helm, Peter A.; van Leeuwen, Cees (November 2012). "A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: II Conceptual and theoretical foundations". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1218–1252. doi:10.1037/a0029334. ISSN 0033-2909. PMC 3728284. PMID 22845750.
  8. ^ Rubin, E. (1921) Visuell Wahrgenommene Figuren : Studien in psychologischer Analyse. Kobenhaven: Gyldendalske boghandel.
  9. ^ Rubin, E. (2001). Figure and Ground. In Yantis, S.(Ed.), Visual Perception. (pp. 225-229). Philadelphia, Psychology Press
  10. ^ Schacter, L.D., Gilbert, D.T., Wegner, D.M. (2011). "Psychology (2nd ed.)." New York, NY: Worth Publishers
  11. ^ Hasson, U., Hendler, T., Ben Bashat, D., and Malach, R. (2001). Vase or face? A neural correlate of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 744–753.
  12. ^ "Figure 5: The young girl-old woman illusion (otherwise known as "My..." ResearchGate. Retrieved 2019-12-10.
  13. ^ cc, 21 Digital Web Solutions. "Early learning: Figure Ground Development in baby's and toddlers". Educanda. Archived from the original on 2019-12-11. Retrieved 2019-12-11.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  14. ^ Quinn, Paul C.; Bhatt, Ramesh S. (2018-08-28). "Size and orientation cue figure-ground segregation in infants". Visual Cognition. 26 (7): 518–529. doi:10.1080/13506285.2018.1505794. PMC 6786798. PMID 31602175.
  15. ^ Quinn, Paul C.; Bhatt, Ramesh S. (2018-08-28). "Size and orientation cue figure-ground segregation in infants". Visual Cognition. 26 (7): 518–529. doi:10.1080/13506285.2018.1505794. PMC 6786798. PMID 31602175.
  16. ^ cc, 21 Digital Web Solutions. "Early learning: Figure Ground Development in baby's and toddlers". Educanda. Archived from the original on 2019-12-11. Retrieved 2019-12-11.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.; Daniel T., Gilbert; Daniel M., Wegner (March 2011). Psychology: Second Edition. United States of America: Worth Publishers. pp. 149. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  18. ^ Pitts, Michael A.; Martínez, Antígona; Brewer, James B.; Hillyard, Steven A. (April 2011). "Early stages of figure-ground segregation during perception of the face-vase". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 23 (4): 880–895. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21438. ISSN 0898-929X. PMC 3071650. PMID 20146604.
  19. ^ Molloy, Katharine; Lavie, Nilli; Chait, Maria (2019-02-27). "Auditory figure-ground segregation is impaired by high visual load". The Journal of Neuroscience. 39 (9): 1699–1708. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2518-18.2018. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6391559. PMID 30541915.
  20. ^ Goldreich, D; Peterson, MA (2012). "A Bayesian observer replicates convexity context effects in figure–ground perception". Seeing and Perceiving. 25 (3–4): 365–95. doi:10.1163/187847612X634445. PMID 22564398.
  21. ^ Barghout, L; Lee, L (2003). "Perceptual information processing system". USPatent Patent Application 10/618,543.
  22. ^ Peterson, Mary; Salvagio, Elizabeth (2010). "Figure-ground perception". Scholarpedia. 5 (4): 4320. Bibcode:2010SchpJ...5.4320P. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.4320. ISSN 1941-6016.
  23. ^ Carlson, Neil R (2010). Psychology the science of behaviour-figure and ground. US: Pearson Canada. pp. 175. ISBN 978-0-205-64524-4.
  24. ^ Tóth, Brigitta; Kocsis, Zsuzsanna; Háden, Gábor P.; Szerafin, Ágnes; Shinn-Cunningham, Barbara G.; Winkler, István (May 2018). "Corrigendum to "EEG signatures accompanying auditory figure-ground segregation" [NeuroImage (2017) volume 141, pp. 108–119]". NeuroImage. 172: 915. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.001. ISSN 1053-8119. PMC 8963124. PMID 29233621.
  25. ^ Teki, S.; Chait, M.; Kumar, S.; von Kriegstein, K.; Griffiths, T. D. (2011-01-05). "Brain Bases for Auditory Stimulus-Driven Figure-Ground Segregation". Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (1): 164–171. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3788-10.2011. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 3059575. PMID 21209201.
  26. ^ "Tulane University Central Authentication Service 1p01". cas.tulane.edu. Archived from the original on 2019-12-17. Retrieved 2019-12-10.
  27. ^ Molloy, Katharine; Lavie, Nilli; Chait, Maria (2019-02-27). "Auditory figure-ground segregation is impaired by high visual load". The Journal of Neuroscience. 39 (9): 1699–1708. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2518-18.2018. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6391559. PMID 30541915.
  28. ^ Houtkamp, R.; Roelfsema, P. R. (2004-08-01). "Figure-ground and figure-figure segregation in curve tracing". Journal of Vision. 4 (8): 199. doi:10.1167/4.8.199. ISSN 1534-7362.
  29. ^ Lohr L. Creating Graphics for Learning and Performance: Lessons in Visual Literacy Cleveland, OH: Prentice-Hall, 2007.
  • Figure Ground, a puzzle game plays on the figure–ground illusion.

Read other articles:

American football player (born 1999) American football player Cesar RuizRuiz with Michigan in 2018No. 51 – New Orleans SaintsPosition:GuardPersonal informationBorn: (1999-06-14) June 14, 1999 (age 24)Camden, New Jersey, U.S.Height:6 ft 4 in (1.93 m)Weight:316 lb (143 kg)Career informationHigh school:IMG Academy(Bradenton, Florida)College:Michigan (2017–2019)NFL Draft:2020 / Round: 1 / Pick: 24Career history New Orleans Saints (2020–prese...

Провідни́к Windows Компонент Microsoft Windows ДеталіУ комплекті з Microsoft Windows 95 та даліЗамінює Диспетчер файлівДиспетчер програмРедактор PIFПов'язані компоненти Меню «Пуск» Провідни́к Windows (англ. Windows Explorer) — програма Explorer.exe, навігаційний файловий менеджер, міститься у каталозі о�...

Instituto Nacional sobre el Abuso de Drogas Tipo organismo públicoSede central Bethesda (Estados Unidos)Empresa matriz National Institutes of HealthSitio web www.drugabuse.gov y nida.nih.gov[editar datos en Wikidata] El National Institute on Drug Abuse o NIDA (en español: Instituto Nacional sobre el Abuso de Drogas[1]​) es una institución del gobierno estadounidense cuya misión es avanzar en el conocimiento de las causas y consecuencias del uso y la adicción a las drogas,...

Peta yang menunjukkan letak Pigkawayan Pigcawayan adalah munisipalitas di provinsi Cotabato, Filipina. Pada tahun 2009, Pigcawayan memiliki populasi sebanyak 60.398 jiwa atau 13.778 rumah tangga. Pembagian wilayah Secara politis Pigcawayan terbagi atas 40 barangay. Anick (Upper Balogo) Upper Baguer (Baguer) Balacayon Balogo Banucagon Bulucaon Buluan Buricain Capayuran Central Panatan Datu Binasing Datu Mantil Kadingilan Kimarayag Libungan Torreta Lower Baguer Lower Pangangkalan Malagakit Malu...

Artikel ini sebatang kara, artinya tidak ada artikel lain yang memiliki pranala balik ke halaman ini.Bantulah menambah pranala ke artikel ini dari artikel yang berhubungan atau coba peralatan pencari pranala.Tag ini diberikan pada Agustus 2020. Artikel atau sebagian dari artikel ini mungkin diterjemahkan dari Ryan Creamer di en.wikipedia.org. Isinya masih belum akurat, karena bagian yang diterjemahkan masih perlu diperhalus dan disempurnakan. Jika Anda menguasai bahasa aslinya, harap pertimba...

Former stadium in Regina, Saskatchewan, Canada This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Taylor Field Regina, Saskatchewan – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (June 2015) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Taylor FieldMosaic Stadium in its final configuration.Former nam...

German field hockey player (born 1986) Nina Hasselmann 2010 Women's Hockey Champions Trophy England vs. Germany Bronze medal matchPersonal informationBorn (1986-05-31) 31 May 1986 (age 37)Nürnberg, West GermanyHeight 160 cm (5 ft 3 in)Weight 57 kg (126 lb)National teamYears Team Apps (Gls)2005 Germany U21 8 (1)2008-2016 Germany 219 (7) Nina Hasselmann (born 31 May 1986, Nürnberg, West Germany)[1] is a German field hockey player. At the 2012 Summer Olymp...

Образование в Финляндии (фин. Koulutus Suomessa) — сеть образовательных учреждений на территории Финляндии. По данным Центра статистики на конец 2011 года в стране действовало 3934 учебных заведения, в которых числилось 1,94 млн учащихся[1]. Среди 29 университетов десять так н�...

16th episode of the 1st season of Glee HomeGlee episodeEpisode no.Season 1Episode 16Directed byParis BarclayWritten byBrad FalchukFeatured musicFireA House Is Not a HomeOne Less Bell to Answer / A House Is Not a HomeBeautifulHomeProduction code1ARC15Original air dateApril 27, 2010 (2010-04-27)Guest appearances Jonathan Groff as Jesse St. James Kristin Chenoweth as April Rhodes Mike O'Malley as Burt Hummel Romy Rosemont as Carole Hudson Michael Benjamin Washington as Tracey...

Ethnic group in Transcaucasia For other uses, see Lom (disambiguation). Not to be confused with Loma people. Lom BoshaBosha gypsies, 19th centuryRegions with significant populationsArmenia, Georgia and TurkeyLanguagesLomavren, Armenian, Georgian, TurkishReligionChristianityRelated ethnic groupsRomani, Doms, Domba, Ghorbati; other Indo-Aryans The Lom people (Turkish: Lomlar), also known by non-Loms as Bosha or Posha (Turkish: Poşa; Armenian: Բոշա; Georgian: ბოშა, romanized: bo...

Дважды наращённая треугольная призма (3D-модель) Тип многогранник Джонсона Свойства выпуклая Комбинаторика Элементы 11 граней17 рёбер8 вершин Χ = 2 Грани 10 треугольников1 квадрат Конфигурация вершины 2(35) 2(34) 4(33.4) Развёртка Классификация Обозначения J50, П3+2М2 Гру�...

American comedy television series Not to be confused with Heil Honey I'm Home!. This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Hi Honey, I'm Home! – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (June 2017) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Hi Honey, I'm Home!Created byRick MitzPenny Stalli...

Second largest bank in Germany by asset size You can help expand this article with text translated from the corresponding article in German. (August 2016) Click [show] for important translation instructions. Machine translation, like DeepL or Google Translate, is a useful starting point for translations, but translators must revise errors as necessary and confirm that the translation is accurate, rather than simply copy-pasting machine-translated text into the English Wikipedia. Consider...

Railway station in Essex, England West HorndonWest Horndon railway station in 1991General informationLocationWest Horndon, Borough of BrentwoodEnglandCoordinates51°34′04″N 0°20′26″E / 51.5679°N 0.3406°E / 51.5679; 0.3406Grid referenceTQ623880Owned byNetwork RailManaged byc2cPlatforms2Other informationStation codeWHRClassificationDfT category EHistoryOriginal companyLondon, Tilbury and Southend RailwayPre-groupingMidland RailwayPost-groupingLondon, Midland a...

Lago de Caicedo Yuso Vista del lago desde el oesteUbicación geográficaContinente EuropaCoordenadas 42°46′42″N 2°59′28″O / 42.77833333, -2.99111111Ubicación administrativaPaís España EspañaDivisión LantarónLocalidad Caicedo de YusoCuerpo de aguaSuperficie 6,5 ha.Profundidad Máxima: 25 m.[editar datos en Wikidata] El lago de Arreo o lago de Caicedo Yuso es el lago natural más importante del País Vasco,[1]​[2]​ situado en las inmedi...

NJ Transit rail station MetuchenLooking towards New York on the inbound platform.General informationLocationMain Street and Woodbridge AvenueMetuchen, New JerseyUnited StatesCoordinates40°32′26.70″N 74°21′37.22″W / 40.5407500°N 74.3603389°W / 40.5407500; -74.3603389Owned byNew Jersey TransitLine(s)Amtrak Northeast CorridorPlatforms2 side platformsTracks4Connections NJ Transit Bus: 810, 813, 819ConstructionAccessibleYesOther informationFare zone11HistoryOpen...

For the company, see PV Crystalox Solar. PVCS Version ManagerOriginal author(s)PolytronDeveloper(s)Micro FocusInitial release1985; 38 years ago (1985)Stable release8.6.3 / February 2021; 2 years ago (2021-02)[1] Written inC, C++, JavaOperating systemDOS,[citation needed] Windows, Unix-likeTypeVersion controlLicenseProprietary softwareWebsitewww.microfocus.com/products/pvcs/  PVCS Version Manager (originally named Polytron Version C...

South Korean judoka Kim Min-jeongPersonal informationBorn (1988-08-08) 8 August 1988 (age 35)OccupationJudokaHeight1.79 m (5 ft 10 in)Weight95 kg (209 lb)SportCountry South KoreaSportJudoWeight class+78 kgAchievements and titlesOlympic Games5th (2016)World Champ. (2017)Asian Champ. (2016, 2019) Medal record Women's judo Representing  South Korea World Championships 2017 Budapest +78 kg Asian Games 2014 Incheon Women's team 2018 ...

هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إليها في مقالات متعلقة بها. (أبريل 2016) عمر غازي معلومات شخصية الميلاد سنة 1984 (العمر 38–39 سنة)  تعديل مصدري - تعديل   عمر غازي عواقلة لاعب كرة قدم أردني محترف من مواليد عام 1984، يلعب حاليا في خط �...

1999 Indian filmMalabar PolicePosterDirected byP. VasuWritten byP. VasuProduced byShanthi VasudevanStarringSathyarajKhushbuAbbasMumtajCinematographyAshok KumarEdited byP. MohanrajMusic byS. A. RajkumarProductioncompanyKamalam MoviesRelease date 6 August 1999 (1999-08-06) Running time150 minutesCountryIndiaLanguageTamil Malabar Police is a 1999 Indian Tamil-language crime comedy film written and directed by P. Vasu. The film stars Sathyaraj, Khushbu, Abbas and Mumtaj. It was rel...