Surround suppression

Surround suppression is where the relative firing rate of a neuron may under certain conditions decrease when a particular stimulus is enlarged. It has been observed in electrophysiology studies of the brain and has been noted in many sensory neurons, most notably in the early visual system. Surround suppression is defined as a reduction in the activity of a neuron in response to a stimulus outside its classical receptive field.

The necessary functional connections with other neurons influenced by stimulation outside a particular area and by dynamic processes in general, and the absence of a theoretical description of a system state to be treated as a baseline, deprive the term "classical receptive field" of functional meaning.[1] The descriptor "surround suppression" suffers from a similar problem, as the activities of neurons in the "surround" of the "classical receptive field are similarly determined by connectivities and processes involving neurons beyond it.) This nonlinear effect is one of many that reveals the complexity of biological sensory systems, and the connections of properties of neurons that may cause this effect (or its opposite) are still being studied. The characteristics, mechanisms, and perceptual consequences of this phenomenon are of interest to many communities, including neurobiology, computational neuroscience, psychology, and computer vision.

Background

Stimulation of the surround with the classical receptive field induces a response that is smaller than stimulation of the classical receptive field alone. Stimulation of the surround only has no effect.

The classical model of early vision presumes that each neuron responds independently to a specific stimulus in a localized area of the visual field. (According to Carandini et al (2005), this computational model, which may be fit to various datasets, "degrade[s] quickly if we change almost any aspect of the test stimulus.") The stimulus and corresponding location in the visual field are collectively called the classical receptive field. However, not all effects can be explained by via ad hoc independent filters. Surround suppression is one of an infinite number of possible effects in which neurons do not behave according to the classical model. These effects are collectively called non-classical receptive field effects, and have recently become a substantial research area in vision and other sensory systems.

During surround suppression, neurons are inhibited by a stimulus outside their classical receptive field, in an area loosely termed deemed the 'surround.'

Characteristics: effects on neural responses

Electrophysiology studies

Electrophysiology studies are used to characterize the surround suppression effect. Vision researchers that record neural activity in the primary visual cortex (V1) have seen that spike rates, or neural responses, can be suppressed in as many as 90% of neurons[2][3] by stimuli outside of their surround. In these cells, the spike rates may be reduced by as much as 70%.[4]

Stimulus and attention dependence

Suppression often increases with stimulus size beyond the classical receptive field.

The suppressive effect is often dependent on the contrast, orientation, and direction of motion of the stimulus stimulating the surround. These properties are highly dependent on the brain area and the individual neuron being studied. In MT, for instance, cells can be sensitive to the direction and velocity of stimuli up to 50 to 100 times the area of their classical receptive fields.[5] The statistical properties of the stimuli used to probe these neurons affect the properties of the surround as well. Because these areas are so highly interconnected, stimulation of one cell can affect the response properties of other cells, and therefore researchers have become increasingly aware of the choice of stimuli they use in these experiments. In addition to studies with simple stimuli (dots, bars, sinusoidal gratings),[4][6][7] more recent studies have used more realistic stimuli (natural scenes) to study these effects.[8] Stimuli that better represent natural scenes tend to induce higher levels of suppression, indicating this effect is tied closely to the properties of natural scenes such as ideas and local context.

Surround suppression is also modulated by attention. By training monkeys to attend to certain areas of their visual field, researchers have studied how directed attention can enhance the suppressive effects of stimuli surrounding the area of attention.[9] Similar perceptual studies haven’t been performed on human subjects as well.

Systems involved

Surround suppression was formally discovered in the visual pathway, which has been noticed first by Hubel and Wiesel[6] while mapping receptive fields. The earliest parts of the visual pathway: the retina, Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN), and primary visual cortex (V1) are among the most well-studied. Surround suppression has been studied in later areas as well, including V2, V3, V4,[3] and MT.[10]

Surround suppression has also been seen in sensory systems other than vision. One example in somatosensation is surround suppression in the barrel cortex of mice, in which bending one whisker can suppress the response of a neuron responding to a whisker nearby.[11] It has even been seen in the frequency response properties of electoreception in electric fish.[12]

Biological mechanisms

The biological mechanisms behind surround suppression are not known.[11]

The differences between lateral, feedforward, and recurrent connections

Several theories have been proposed for the biological basis of this effect. Based on the diversity of the stimulus characteristics that cause this effect and the variety of responses that are generated, it seems that many mechanisms may be at play. The most known theory is that it is almost a trial and deduction in your brain.

Lateral connections

Lateral connections are connections between neurons in the same layer. There are many of these connections in all areas of the visual system, which means that a neuron representing one piece of the visual field can influence a neuron representing another piece. Even within lateral connections, there are potentially different mechanisms at play. Monocular mechanisms, requiring stimulation in only one eye, may drive this effect with stimuli with high spatial frequency. When the stimulus frequency is lowered, however, binocular mechanisms come into play, where neurons from different eyes may suppress each other.[13] Model based on this idea have been shown to reproduce surround suppressive effects.

Recurrent feedback

It has been posited that lateral connections are too slow and cover too little of the visual field to fully explain surround suppression.[14] Feedback from higher areas may explain the discrepancies seen in mechanism for surround suppression based purely on lateral connections. There is evidence that inactivation of higher order areas results in reduced strength of surround suppression.[14] At least one model of excitatory connections from higher levels has been formed in the effort to more fully explain surround suppression.[15] However, recurrent feedback is difficult to determine using electrophysiology, and the potential mechanisms at play are not as well studied as feedforward or lateral connections.

Advantages

Surround suppression behavior (and its opposite) gives the sensory system several advantages from both a perceptual and information theory standpoint.

Perceptual advantages

Surround suppression likely participates in context-dependent perceptual tasks. Some specific tasks in which surround suppression may aid include:

  • Motion[4] and velocity[16] detection: In areas such as MT and even V1, the selectivity of neurons to the motion of contrasts may play a potential role in representing the structure of moving objects.
  • Contour integration:[17] Detecting continuity of curved and/or 'broken' edges
  • Texture segregation[18]
  • Perceptual constancies:[3] Recognizing continuity in objects despite changes in lighting, color, or size.
  • Figure-ground segmentation:[19] In this process, local contrast must be used to identify and assign borders.
  • Depth perception (through motion parallax)[3]

These tasks require the use of inputs over wide regions of visual space, meaning that independent responses to small parts of the visual field (a classical linear model of V1) would not be able to produce these effects. There is evidence that surround suppression participates in these tasks by either adjusting the representation of the classical receptive field or representing entirely different features that include both the classical receptive field and the surround. Direct comparison between physiology and psychophysical experiments have been done on several perceptual effects. These include: (1) the reduced apparent contrast of a grating texture embedded in a surrounding grating, (2) target identification when flanked by other features, (3) saliency of broken contours surrounded by edge segments of different orientations, and (4) orientation discrimination when surrounded by features of different orientations and spatial frequencies.[20]

Information theoretic advantages

It has recently been shown that stimulation of the surround may support the efficient coding hypothesis proposed by Horace Barlow in 1961.[21] This hypothesis suggests that the goal of the sensory system is to create an efficient representation of the stimulus. Recently, this has intersected with the idea of a 'sparse' code, one that is represented using the fewest units possible. It has been shown that surround suppression increases the efficiency of transmitting visual information, and may form a sparse code.[22] If many cells respond to parts of the same stimulus, for instance, a lot of redundant information is encoded.[23] The cell needs metabolic energy for each action potential it produces. Therefore, surround suppression likely helps to produce a neural code that is more metabolically efficient. There are additional theoretical advantages, including the removal of statistical redundancy inherent in natural scene statistics, as well as decorrelation of neural responses,[8] which means less information to process later in the pathway.

Edge detection based on center (red) and off center (green) receptive fields

The goal of computer vision is to perform automated tasks similar to those of the human visual system, quickly and accurately interpreting the world and making decisions based on visual information. Because surround suppression seems to play a role in efficient and accurate perception, there have been a few computer vision algorithms inspired by this happening in human vision:

  • Efficient image representation[24]
  • Contoured detection algorithm[25]

So far, the scientific community has been focused on the response properties of the neurons, but exploration of the relation to inference and learning has begun as well.[26]

References

  1. ^ The classical receptive field refers to a concept of neural behavior that was understood to be invalid virtually from the start. As Spillman et al (2015), quoting Kuffler (1953), "not only the areas from which responses can actually be set up by retinal illumination may be included in a definition of the receptive field but also all areas which show a functional connection, by an inhibitory or excitatory effect on a ganglion cell."
  2. ^ Cavanaugh, JR; Bair, W; Movshon, JA (2002a). "Nature and interaction of signals from the receptive field center and surround in macaque V1 neurons" (PDF). J Neurophysiol. 88 (5): 2530–2546. doi:10.1152/jn.00692.2001. PMID 12424292.
  3. ^ a b c d Allman, J; Miezin, F; McGuinness, E (1985). "Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field : Neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons". Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 8: 407–30. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.002203. PMID 3885829.
  4. ^ a b c Jones, H. E.; Grieve, K. L.; Wang, W.; Sillito, A. M. (2001). "Surround Suppression in Primate V1 Surround Suppression in Primate V1". J Neurophysiol. 86 (4): 2011–2028. doi:10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.2011. PMID 11600658.
  5. ^ Allman, J; Miezin, F; McGuinness, E (1985). "Direction- and velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field in the middle temporal visual area (MT)". Perception. 14 (2): 105–126. doi:10.1068/p140105. PMID 4069941. S2CID 8810829.
  6. ^ a b Hubel, D. H.; Wiesel, T. N. (1965). "Receptive Fields and Functional Architecture in Two Nonstriate Visual Areas (18 and 19) of the Cat". Journal of Neurophysiology. 28 (2): 229–89. doi:10.1152/jn.1965.28.2.229. PMID 14283058.
  7. ^ Ozeki, H.; Finn, I. M.; Schaffer, E. S.; Miller, K. D.; Ferster, D. (2009). "Inhibitory stabilization of the cortical network underlies visual surround suppression". Neuron. 62 (4): 578–592. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.028. PMC 2691725. PMID 19477158.
  8. ^ a b Haider, B.; Krause, M. R.; Duque, A.; Yu, Y.; Touryan, J.; Mazer; McCormick (2010). "Synaptic and network mechanisms of sparse and reliable visual cortical activity during nonclassical receptive field stimulation". Neuron. 65 (1): 107–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.005. PMC 3110675. PMID 20152117.
  9. ^ Sundberg, Mitchell; Reynolds (2009). "Spatial Attention Modulates Center-Surround Interactions in Macaque Visual Area V4". Neuron. 61 (6): 952–963. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.023. PMC 3117898. PMID 19324003.
  10. ^ Hunter, J. N.; Born, R. T. (2011). "Stimulus-dependent modulation of suppressive influences in MT". The Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (2): 678–86. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4560-10.2011. PMC 3292058. PMID 21228177.
  11. ^ a b Sachdev, R. N. S.; Krause, M. R.; Mazer, J. A. (2012). "Surround suppression and sparse coding in visual and barrel cortices". Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 6: 43. doi:10.3389/fncir.2012.00043. PMC 3389675. PMID 22783169.
  12. ^ Chacron, M. J.; Doiron, B.; Maler, L.; Longtin, A.; Bastian, J. (2003). "Non-classical receptive field mediates switch in a sensory neuron's frequency tuning". Nature. 423 (6935): 77–81. Bibcode:2003Natur.423...77C. doi:10.1038/nature01590. PMID 12721628. S2CID 4428737.
  13. ^ Webb, B. S.; Dhruv, N. T.; Solomon, S. G.; Tailby, C.; Lennie, P. (2005). "Early and late mechanisms of surround suppression in striate cortex of macaque". The Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (50): 11666–75. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3414-05.2005. PMC 6726034. PMID 16354925.
  14. ^ a b Angelucci, A.; Bullier, J. (2003). "Reaching beyond the classical receptive field of V1 neurons: horizontal or feedback axons?". Journal of Physiology, Paris. 97 (2–3): 141–54. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2003.09.001. PMID 14766139. S2CID 12590563.
  15. ^ Sullivan, T. J.; de Sa, V. R. (2006). "A model of surround suppression through cortical feedback". Neural Networks. 19 (5): 564–72. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.107.6077. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2005.12.003. PMID 16500076.
  16. ^ Perrone, J. A. (2012). "A neural-based code for computing image velocity from small sets of middle temporal (MT/V5) neuron inputs". Journal of Vision. 12 (8): 8. doi:10.1167/12.8.1. PMID 22854102.
  17. ^ Hess, R.; Field, D. (1999). "Integration of contours: new insights". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3 (12): 480–486. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01410-2. PMID 10562727. S2CID 10713134.
  18. ^ Field, D. J.; Hayes, A.; Hess, R. F. (1993). "Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local "association field". Vision Research. 33 (2): 173–93. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(93)90156-q. PMID 8447091. S2CID 205016575.
  19. ^ Supèr, H; Romeo, A; Keil, M (2010). "Feed-Forward Segmentation of Figure-Ground and Assignment of Border-Ownership". PLOS ONE. 5 (5): e10705. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...510705S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010705. PMC 2873306. PMID 20502718.
  20. ^ Seriès, P.; Lorenceau, J.; Frégnac, Y. (2004). "The "silent" surround of V1 receptive fields: theory and experiments" (PDF). Journal of Physiology, Paris. 97 (4–6): 453–74. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.01.023. hdl:20.500.11820/92eaee8b-9d65-490c-af66-b1ad61ef4fad. PMID 15242657. S2CID 9372795.
  21. ^ Barlow, H. B. (1961). Possible principles underlying the transformation of sensory messages. Sensory communication, 217-234.
  22. ^ Vinje, W. E.; Gallant, J. L. (2002). "Natural stimulation of the nonclassical receptive field increases information transmission efficiency in V1". The Journal of Neuroscience. 22 (7): 2904–15. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02904.2002. PMC 6758304. PMID 11923455.
  23. ^ Schwartz, O., & Simoncelli, E. (2001). Natural signal statistics and sensory gain control. Nature neuroscience 819–825. Retrieved from http://www.cns.nyu.edu/csh04/Articles/Schwartz-Simoncelli-01.pdf.
  24. ^ Wei, H.; Wang, Z.; Zuo, Q. (2012). "A Model of Image Representation Based on Non-classical Receptive Fields". Advances in Neural Networks – ISNN 2012. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 7368. pp. 297–306. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-31362-2_34. ISBN 978-3-642-31361-5.
  25. ^ Grigorescu, C.; Petkov, N.; Westenberg (2003). "Contour detection based on nonclassical receptive field inhibition". IEEE Transactions on Image Processing. 12 (7): 729–39. Bibcode:2003ITIP...12..729G. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.162.149. doi:10.1109/TIP.2003.814250. PMID 18237948.
  26. ^ Hyvärinen, A (2010). "Statistical Models of Natural Images and Cortical Visual Representation". Topics in Cognitive Science. 2 (2): 251–264. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.161.6629. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01057.x. PMID 25163788.

Read other articles:

F-16 Fighting Falcon Nampak F-16C Angkatan Udara Amerika Serikat terbang melintasi padang gurun di Irak pada tahun 2008 Jenis Pesawat tempur multiperan Negara asal Amerika Serikat Pembuat General Dynamics (1974–1993)Lockheed Corporation (1993–1995)Lockheed Martin (1995–sekarang) Penerbangan perdana 20 Januari 1974; 50 tahun lalu (1974-01-20) (tidak terencana)2 Februari 1974; 50 tahun lalu (1974-02-02) (resmi) Diperkenalkan 17 Agustus 1978; 45 tahun lalu (1978-08-17) Stat...

 

1st century CE High Priest of Israel Mattathias ben Theophilusמתתיהו בן תאופילוסBornJerusalem, Judea, Roman EmpireDiedc. AD 66Jerusalem, Judea, Roman EmpireParentTheophilus Part of a series of articles onPriesthood in Judaism  KohenPresumption of priestly descentPriestly covenant RolesPriestly BlessingRedemption of the firstborn sonTzaraath (skin disease and mildew)The Torah instruction of the KohanimSacrificeIncense offeringBeth din shel KohanimPriestly divisions H...

 

الرحيق المختوم الرحيق المختوم في السيرة النبوية غلاف من إحدى طبعات الرحيق المختوم معلومات الكتاب المؤلف صفي الرحمن المباركفوري البلد السعودية اللغة اللغة العربية الناشر أولي النهى للإنتاج الإعلامي تاريخ النشر 1422 هـ الموضوع السيرة النبوية، تاريخ التقديم عدد الصفحات 512 ص...

Comic book album The Sandman: Brief LivesCover of The Sandman: Brief Lives  (1994), trade paperback collected edition, art by Dave McKeanPublisherDC ComicsPublication dateSeptember 1992 – May 1993Title(s)The Sandman #41–49Main character(s)DreamISBNISBN 1-85286-577-6Creative teamWriter(s)Neil GaimanPenciller(s)Jill ThompsonInker(s)Vince LockeDick GiordanoLetterer(s)Todd KleinColorist(s)Daniel VozzoEditor(s)Karen BergerLisa AufenangerAlisa Kwitney Brief Lives (1994) is the seventh...

 

Aire d'attraction de Crest Localisation de l'aire d'attraction de Crest dans le département de la Drôme. Géographie Pays France Région Auvergne-Rhône-Alpes Département Drôme Caractéristiques Type Aire d'attraction d'une ville Code Insee 342 Catégorie Aires de moins de 50 000 habitants Nombre de communes 17 Population 18 081 hab. (2021) modifier  L'aire d'attraction de Crest est un zonage d'étude défini par l'Insee pour caractériser l’influence de la com...

 

Television channel Rangers TVOwnershipOwnerRangers Football Club PLCSetanta Sports (TV channel)HistoryLaunched26 July 2004 (Channel)30 October 2009 (Online Service)Closed23 June 2009 (Channel)LinksWebsiterangerstv.tv Rangers TV is a British premium streaming service devoted to coverage of the Scottish football club Rangers F.C. From 2004-2009, Celtic TV existed as a sports television channel operated by Irish sports broadcasting company Setanta Sports. History Launched in July 2004 alongside...

イスラームにおける結婚(イスラームにおけるけっこん)とは、二者の間で行われる法的な契約である。新郎新婦は自身の自由な意思で結婚に同意する。口頭または紙面での規則に従った拘束的な契約は、イスラームの結婚で不可欠だと考えられており、新郎と新婦の権利と責任の概要を示している[1]。イスラームにおける離婚は様々な形をとることができ、個�...

 

Untuk orang lain dengan nama yang sama, lihat Sulochana. SulochanaSulochana pada 1920an.LahirRuby Myers1907Pune, Kepresidenan Bombay, India BritaniaMeninggal10 Oktober 1983 [1]Mumbai, Maharashtra, IndiaPekerjaanaktorTahun aktif1920an–1980an Ruby Myers (1907 – 10 Oktober 1983), yang lebih dikenal dengan nama panggungnya Sulochana (सुलोचना), adalah seorang bintang film bisu India keturunan Yahudi, yang berasal dari komunitas Yahudi Baghdad di India (lihat Sejarah ...

 

Serie B 2018-2019 Competizione Serie B Sport Calcio Edizione 48ª Organizzatore FIGC - Divisione Calcio Femminile Date dal 14 ottobre 2018al 5 maggio 2019 Luogo  Italia Partecipanti 12 Formula Girone all'italiana A/R Risultati Vincitore Inter(1º titolo) Promozioni InterEmpoli Retrocessioni Genoa WomenArezzoRoma XIV Statistiche Miglior marcatore Gloria Marinelli (26) Incontri disputati 132 Gol segnati 452 (3,42 per incontro) Cronologia della competizione 2017-2018 2019...

追晉陸軍二級上將趙家驤將軍个人资料出生1910年 大清河南省衛輝府汲縣逝世1958年8月23日(1958歲—08—23)(47—48歲) † 中華民國福建省金門縣国籍 中華民國政党 中國國民黨获奖 青天白日勳章(追贈)军事背景效忠 中華民國服役 國民革命軍 中華民國陸軍服役时间1924年-1958年军衔 二級上將 (追晉)部队四十七師指挥東北剿匪總司令部參謀長陸軍�...

 

J. B. PritzkerPritzker pada 2019 Gubernur Illinois ke-43PetahanaMulai menjabat 14 Januari 2019WakilJuliana StrattonPendahuluBruce RaunerPenggantiPetahana Informasi pribadiLahirJay Robert Pritzker19 Januari 1965 (umur 59)Atherton, California, ASPartai politikPartai DemokratSuami/istriMary Muenster ​(m. 1993)​Anak2KerabatKeluarga PritzkerTempat tinggalGovernor's MansionPendidikanDuke University (Sarjana)Northwestern University (Yuris Doktor)Situs webSitus w...

 

Pour les articles homonymes, voir Canardo. Cet article est une ébauche concernant un coureur cycliste espagnol. Vous pouvez partager vos connaissances en l’améliorant (comment ?). Pour plus d’informations, voyez le projet cyclisme. Mariano CañardoInformationsSurnom El català d’OliteNaissance 5 février 1906Olite-ErriberriDécès 20 juin 1987 (à 81 ans)BarceloneNationalité espagnoleÉquipes professionnelles 1927FC Barcelone1928Elvish-Wolber1929FC Barcelone1930Styl1934-19...

Fourth Division 1986-1987Today Fourth Division 1986-1987 Competizione Fourth Division Sport Calcio Edizione 29ª Organizzatore Football League Date dal 23 agosto 1986al 25 maggio 1987 Luogo  Inghilterra Galles Partecipanti 24 Formula girone all'italiana+play off Risultati Vincitore Northampton Town(1º titolo) Altre promozioni Preston North EndSouthend UnitedAldershot (dopo play off) Retrocessioni Lincoln City Statistiche Miglior marcatore Richard Hill (29) Incontri...

 

土库曼斯坦总统土库曼斯坦国徽土库曼斯坦总统旗現任谢尔达尔·别尔德穆哈梅多夫自2022年3月19日官邸阿什哈巴德总统府(Oguzkhan Presidential Palace)機關所在地阿什哈巴德任命者直接选举任期7年,可连选连任首任萨帕尔穆拉特·尼亚佐夫设立1991年10月27日 土库曼斯坦土库曼斯坦政府与政治 国家政府 土库曼斯坦宪法 国旗 国徽 国歌 立法機關(英语:National Council of Turkmenistan) ...

 

North-central African regionAlgerian Desertالصحراء الجزائريةSand dunes of AlgeriaMap of the Algerian arid desertGeographyCountry AlgeriaClimate typeArid desertEl Gour in El Bayadh ProvinceTamanrasset desertThe Algerian Desert (Arabic: الصحراء الجزائرية, romanized: al-Saḥrā' al-Jazā'iriyah) is a desert located in central North Africa within Algeria, constituting part of the Sahara. The desert covers more than 90% of Algeria's total area.[1]...

نصب صلاح الدين الأيوبي   معلومات فنية الفنان عبد الله السيد تاريخ إنشاء العمل 1993  بلد المنشأ  سوريا الموقع دمشق نوع العمل تمثال الموضوع وسيط property غير متوفر. المدينة دمشق المالك محافظة دمشق معلومات أخرى المواد برونز  الطول وسيط property غير متوفر. الوزن وسيط property غير م...

 

Danish film director and screenwriter Bille AugustRDAugust in 2008Born (1948-11-09) 9 November 1948 (age 75)Brede, DenmarkOccupationFilm directorYears active1978–presentSpouses Annie Munksgaard Lauritzen ​ ​(m. 1970; div. 1978)​ Masja Dessau ​ ​(m. 1978; div. 1990)​ Pernilla August ​ ​(m. 1991; div. 1997)​ Sara-Marie Maltha ​(...

 

  ميّز عن ديكساميتازون. ديكستروميثورفان ديكستروميثورفان الاسم النظامي (4bS,8aR,9S)-3-Methoxy-11-methyl-6,7,8,8a,9,10-hexahydro-5H-9,4b-(epiminoethano)phenanthrene تداخل دوائي إيزوكاربوكسازيد،  وفينيلزين،  وبروكاربازين،  و(±)-دبرينيل  [لغات أخرى]‏،  وترانيلسيبرومين[1]  يعالج سعال،  ...

Shah of Iran (r. 1736–47) and founder of the Afsharid dynasty Nadir Shah redirects here. For the 20th-century king of Afghanistan, see Mohammad Nadir Shah. For other people with the same name, see Nadir Shah (disambiguation). Nader Shah King of Kings[1] Sultan of the Sultans of the World[2] Contemporary portrait of Nader Shah. Artist unknown, created in c. 1740 in Iran. Now located in the British Library in London.Shah of IranReign8 March 1736 – 20 June 1747[3]Coro...

 

Australian singing competition TV series For the most recent season, see Australian Idol season 9. This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Australian Idol – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (November 2011) (Learn how and when to remove this message) Australian IdolCreated bySimon FullerPre...