Ostrich effect

The ostrich effect, also known as the ostrich problem,[1] was originally coined by Galai & Sade (2003).[2] The name comes from the common (but false) legend that ostriches bury their heads in the sand to avoid danger. This effect is a cognitive bias where people tend to “bury their head in the sand” and avoid potentially negative but useful information, such as feedback on progress, to avoid psychological discomfort.[1]

Neuroscientific evidence

There is neuroscientific evidence of the ostrich effect. Sharot et al. (2012) investigated the differences in positive and negative information when updating existing beliefs. Consistent with the ostrich effect, participants presented with negative information were more likely to avoid updating their beliefs. Moreover, they found that the part of the brain responsible for this cognitive bias was the left IFG - by disrupting this part of the brain with TMS, participants were more likely to accept the negative information provided.[3]

Researched contexts & applications

Finance

An everyday example of the ostrich effect in a financial context is people avoiding checking their bank account balance after spending a lot of money.[4] The studies below explore the ostrich effect through investors in financial markets.

Galai & Sade (2003) studied investors' decision-making in Israel’s capital market. They found that investors prefer financial investments where the risk is unreported over those with a similar risk-return profile but with frequently reported risks, saying that investors are willing to pay a premium for "the bliss of ignorance".[2]

Later, Karlsson et al. (2005) studied investors’ decision-making in Swedish and US markets. They determined that investors from both countries looked up their portfolios more when the market index was increasing (positive information) and less when the index was decreasing (negative information).[5]

Healthcare

There are known negative implications of the ostrich effect in healthcare. For example, people with diabetes avoid monitoring their blood sugar levels.[6]

Banerjee & Zanella highlighted the ostrich effect in avoiding preventive screening, studying women working at a company to understand how a woman’s propensity to get annual mammograms changes after a co-worker is diagnosed with breast cancer. The company had on-site mammograms and removed all barriers to getting them, such as cost and long queues. 70% of eligible women took up the company’s offer of an annual mammogram. However, surprisingly, in the presence of a co-worker diagnosed with breast cancer, women “spatially closer to her in the workplace” are 8% less likely to get a screening.[7] Highlighting that in the presence of potentially negative information, people tend to avoid the chance to receive it.

Climate and energy

Research has found that when people feel uninformed about a pressing matter, they may exhibit the ostrich effect.[8] The ostrich effect may explain why people sometimes avoid tackling climate change or energy depletion.

Shepherd & Kay (2012) presented participants with a passage. One group read that the US would have oil for 240 more years (positive information), while the other read that supplies would diminish in 40 years (negative information). Afterwards, participants completed a questionnaire to gauge their interest in learning about energy depletion. Those who read that energy depletion was an urgent problem and that oil would run out in 40 years were more likely to avoid learning about the issue.[8]

Theories on causes

Cognitive dissonance

Cognitive dissonance is a state of psychological discomfort that arises when an individual holds two or more conflicting beliefs.[9] Chang et al. (2017) found that when participants ranked reasons on why they did not monitor progress, the main reason was that “information on goal progress would demand a change in beliefs”.[10] This statement shows that when confronted with information that contradicts their beliefs, individuals may experience cognitive dissonance and avoid seeking it to reduce discomfort. This avoidance is the ostrich effect. The opposite, seeking information consistent with your beliefs, is a cognitive bias termed confirmation bias.[11]

Trustability

Chang et al. (2017) also found that some participants exhibited the ostrich effect because they did not trust the information provided.[10] Lack of trust is especially true for negative information; Ilgen et al. (1979) found that people are more likely to trust positive feedback than negative feedback.[12] Additionally, DeBono & Harnish (1988) found that the information's trustability depends on the perceived expertise of the information provider. The higher the perceived expertise, the more likely people trust it.[13]

Loss aversion

Loss aversion is the tendency for people to feel the pain of losses more strongly than the pleasure of equivalent gains.[14] Panidi (2015) looked at the link between loss aversion and the ostrich effect - loss aversion was measured through lottery choices, and the ostrich effect was measured through preventive medical testing. The study found that higher loss aversion decreases the chance of the decision to do a preventive medical test.[15] Demonstrating that the higher the loss aversion in an individual, the more likely they are to display the ostrich effect by avoiding information on diagnosis.

Criticism

Meerkat-Effect

Initial findings

Gherzi et al. (2014) studied 617 investors from Barclays Wealth & Management UK. They found no perceivable attempt by investors to ignore or avoid negative information. Instead, they saw that "investors increase their portfolio monitoring following both positive and daily negative market returns, behaving more like hyper-vigilant meerkats than head-in-the-sand ostriches". They dubbed this phenomenon the "meerkat effect".[16]

Follow-up research

Sicherman et al. (2016) showed that the sample and demographic moderate the extent that investors exhibited the ostrich effect. In a sample of 100,000, Sicherman et al. (2016) found that 79% of investors showed the ostrich effect while 21% had “anti-ostrich behaviour”, such as the meerkat effect.

The researchers argued that Gherzi et al. (2014) sample size of 617 investors was too small, one potential reason that most investors exhibited the meerkat effect rather than the ostrich effect. Sicherman et al. (2016) also showed that the ostrich effect appeared more in “men, older investors and wealthier investors”. [16][17]

Future research: Culture

Another moderator for the ostrich effect that has yet to be specifically studied but has been theorised is cultural differences.[16][17] Culture may impact the ostrich effect as the underlying causes of the ostrich effect are all influenced by culture. Hoshino-Browne et al. (2005) showed that cognitive dissonance is resolved in different manners in collectivistic cultures compared to individualistic cultures.[18] Furthermore, Wang et al. (2016) shows that loss aversion is higher in individualistic cultures,[19] and Rose & Kitayama (1991) found that collectivistic cultures tend to trust negative feedback and reject positive ones.[20] Individualism appears more in western culture,[21] hinting at the ostrich effect being higher in western cultures. The studies on the ostrich effect are predominantly conducted on western cultures; therefore, future studies must test for potential cultural differences in the ostrich effect.

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Webb, Thomas L.; Chang, Betty P. I.; Benn, Yael (2013). "'The Ostrich Problem': Motivated Avoidance or Rejection of Information About Goal Progress". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (11): 794–807. doi:10.1111/spc3.12071. ISSN 1751-9004.
  2. ^ a b Galai, Dan; Sade, Orly (2003). "The 'Ostrich Effect' and the Relationship between the Liquidity and the Yields of Financial Assets". SSRN Electronic Journal. doi:10.2139/ssrn.431180. ISSN 1556-5068. S2CID 154904068.
  3. ^ Sharot, Tali; Kanai, Ryota; Marston, David; Korn, Christoph W.; Rees, Geraint; Dolan, Raymond J. (2012-10-16). "Selectively altering belief formation in the human brain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (42): 17058–17062. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10917058S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1205828109. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3479523. PMID 23011798.
  4. ^ Olafsson, Arna; Pagel, Michaela (2017). "The Ostrich in Us: Selective Attention to Financial Accounts, Income, Spending, and Liquidity" (PDF). National Bureau of Economic Research. Cambridge, MA: w23945. doi:10.3386/w23945. S2CID 158385626.
  5. ^ Karlsson, Niklas; Seppi, Duane J.; Loewenstein, George F. (2005). "The 'Ostrich Effect': Selective Attention to Information about Investments". SSRN Electronic Journal. doi:10.2139/ssrn.772125. ISSN 1556-5068. S2CID 154954897.
  6. ^ Peel, Elizabeth; Douglas, Margaret; Lawton, Julia (2007-09-06). "Self monitoring of blood glucose in type 2 diabetes: longitudinal qualitative study of patients' perspectives". BMJ. 335 (7618): 493. doi:10.1136/bmj.39302.444572.DE. ISSN 0959-8138. PMC 1971180. PMID 17761996.
  7. ^ Zanella, Giulio; Banerjee, Ritesh (2014-03-14). "Experiencing Breast Cancer at the Workplace". Rochester, NY. doi:10.2139/ssrn.2424782. hdl:10419/159777. S2CID 153923349. SSRN 2424782. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  8. ^ a b Shepherd, Steven; Kay, Aaron C. (2012). "On the perpetuation of ignorance: system dependence, system justification, and the motivated avoidance of sociopolitical information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 102 (2): 264–280. doi:10.1037/a0026272. hdl:10012/6813. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 22059846.
  9. ^ Aronson, Elliot (1969-01-01), Berkowitz, Leonard (ed.), "The Theory of Cognitive Dissonance: A Current Perspective11This article and all the original work reported in it were supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation.", Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 4, Academic Press, pp. 1–34, doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60075-1, retrieved 2023-03-31
  10. ^ a b Chang, Betty P. I.; Webb, Thomas L.; Benn, Yael (2017-02-08). "Why Do People Act Like the Proverbial Ostrich? Investigating the Reasons That People Provide for Not Monitoring Their Goal Progress". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 152. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00152. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5297323. PMID 28228740.
  11. ^ Nickerson, Raymond S. (1998). "Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises". Review of General Psychology. 2 (2): 175–220. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175. ISSN 1089-2680. S2CID 8508954.
  12. ^ Ilgen, Daniel R.; Fisher, Cynthia D.; Taylor, M. Susan (1979). "Consequences of individual feedback on behavior in organizations". Journal of Applied Psychology. 64 (4): 349–371. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.64.4.349. ISSN 1939-1854.
  13. ^ DeBono, Kenneth G.; Harnish, Richard J. (1988). "Source expertise, source attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A functional approach". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55 (4): 541–546. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.541. ISSN 1939-1315.
  14. ^ Tversky, Amos; Kahneman, Daniel (1992-10-01). "Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty". Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 5 (4): 297–323. doi:10.1007/BF00122574. ISSN 1573-0476. S2CID 8456150.
  15. ^ Panidi, Ksenia (2015-02-01). "Ostrich Effect in Health Care Decisions: Theory and Empirical Evidence". Rochester, NY. SSRN 2932181. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  16. ^ a b c Gherzi, Svetlana; Egan, Daniel; Stewart, Neil; Haisley, Emily; Ayton, Peter (2014-11-01). "The meerkat effect: Personality and market returns affect investors' portfolio monitoring behaviour". Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. Empirical Behavioral Finance. 107: 512–526. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2014.07.013. ISSN 0167-2681.
  17. ^ a b Sicherman, Nachum; Loewenstein, George; Seppi, Duane J.; Utkus, Stephen P. (2016). "Financial Attention". Review of Financial Studies. 29 (4): 863–897. doi:10.1093/rfs/hhv073. ISSN 0893-9454.
  18. ^ Hoshino-Browne, Etsuko; Zanna, Adam S.; Spencer, Steven J.; Zanna, Mark P.; Kitayama, Shinobu; Lackenbauer, Sandra (2005). "On the cultural guises of cognitive dissonance: The case of Easterners and Westerners". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 89 (3): 294–310. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.294. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 16248715.
  19. ^ Wang, Mei; Rieger, Marc Oliver; Hens, Thorsten (2017). "The Impact of Culture on Loss Aversion: Loss Aversion". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 30 (2): 270–281. doi:10.1002/bdm.1941. S2CID 147620509.
  20. ^ Markus, Hazel R.; Kitayama, Shinobu (1991). "Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation". Psychological Review. 98 (2): 224–253. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.224. ISSN 1939-1471. S2CID 13606371.
  21. ^ Hofstede, Geert (2011-12-01). "Dimensionalizing Cultures: The Hofstede Model in Context". Online Readings in Psychology and Culture. 2 (1). doi:10.9707/2307-0919.1014. ISSN 2307-0919. S2CID 7313352.

Read other articles:

Turnamen putra Bola Voli pada Pesta Olahraga Asia Tenggara 2023LokasiOlympic Complex Indoor Main HallTanggal3–8 MeiNegara8← 20212025 → Artikel utama: Bola Voli di Pesta Olahraga Asia Tenggara 2023 Turnamen bola voli putra di Pesta Olahraga Asia Tenggara 2023 akan diadakan di Kamboja dari 3 Mei hingga 8 Mei 2023. Undian Undian untuk turnamen bola voli putra diadakan pada tanggal 5 April 2023 di Phnom Penh. Kamboja sebagai tuan rumah memilih grup yang ingin dialokasikan...

 

Hinterland Aviation IATA ICAO Kode panggil OI HND HINTERLAND Didirikan11 May 1984 (11 May 1984)Penghubung Bandar Udara Cairns Bandar Udara Townsville Bandar Udara Rockhampton Armada17Tujuan7Kantor pusatCairns, AustraliaSitus webhinterlandaviation.com.au Hinterland Aviation adalah maskapai penerbangan regional dan perusahaan sewaan yang memiliki basis di Bandar Udara Cairns di Queensland, Australia. Maskapai penerbangan ini mengoperasikan penerbangan berjadwal dari Bandar Udara Cairns dan...

 

Italian electronic duo band This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these template messages) This article contains content that is written like an advertisement. Please help improve it by removing promotional content and inappropriate external links, and by adding encyclopedic content written from a neutral point of view. (March 2020) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) This article may r...

1988 novel The Kentish Manor Murders First editionAuthorJulian SymonsCountryUnited KingdomLanguageEnglishGenreDetectivePublisherMacmillanPublication date1988Media typePrintPages191ISBN0140108726Preceded byA Three-Pipe Problem  The Kentish Manor Murders is a 1988 mystery detective novel by the British writer Julian Symons. A pastiche of the traditional Sherlock Holmes stories by Arthur Conan Doyle, it is a sequel to the 1975 novel A Three-Pipe Problem.[1] Plot Sheridan H...

 

Penyuntingan Artikel oleh pengguna baru atau anonim untuk saat ini tidak diizinkan.Lihat kebijakan pelindungan dan log pelindungan untuk informasi selengkapnya. Jika Anda tidak dapat menyunting Artikel ini dan Anda ingin melakukannya, Anda dapat memohon permintaan penyuntingan, diskusikan perubahan yang ingin dilakukan di halaman pembicaraan, memohon untuk melepaskan pelindungan, masuk, atau buatlah sebuah akun. Artikel ini membutuhkan rujukan tambahan agar kualitasnya dapat dipastikan. Mohon...

 

Luna 9Sebuah replika Luna 9 di Museum of Air and Space Paris, Le Bourget.Jenis misiPendarat BulanOperatorGSMZ LavochkinCOSPAR ID1966-006ASATCAT no.01954Durasi misi6 hari, 11 jam, 10 menit Properti wahanaJenis wahana antariksaYe-6ProdusenGSMZ LavochkinMassa luncur1580 kgMassa mendarat99 kg Awal misiTanggal luncur31 Januari 1966, 11:45:00 GMTRoket peluncurMolniya-M 8K78M s/n 103-32Tempat peluncuranBaikonur, Site 31/6 Akhir MisiKontak terakhir6 Februari 1966, 22:55 GMT Parameter orbitSistem ruju...

Voce principale: Coppa delle Coppe 1988-1989. Finale della Coppa delle Coppe 1988-1989Il Wankdorfstadion di Berna, teatro della finaleInformazioni generaliSport Calcio CompetizioneCoppa delle Coppe 1988-1989 Data10 maggio 1989 ImpiantoStadio Wankdorf Spettatori45 000 Dettagli dell'incontro  Barcellona  Sampdoria 2 0 Arbitro George Courtney Successione ← Finale della Coppa delle Coppe 1987-1988 Finale della Coppa delle Coppe 1989-1990 → Modifica dati su Wi...

 

Filipino braised beef stew Pares Top: Beef asado pares, Bottom: Pares kantoPlace of originPhilippinesRegion or stateQuezon CityCreated byLolita TiuInvented1979Serving temperatureHotMain ingredientsClear Soup (commonly beef-based broth) Sinangag Beef Asado (or any other viand)VariationsPares Kanto or Pares Kariton, Pares Mami  Media: Pares Pares (pronounced: PAH-ress), also known as beef pares, is a term for a serving of Filipino braised beef stew with garlic fried rice, and a bowl of...

 

イスラームにおける結婚(イスラームにおけるけっこん)とは、二者の間で行われる法的な契約である。新郎新婦は自身の自由な意思で結婚に同意する。口頭または紙面での規則に従った拘束的な契約は、イスラームの結婚で不可欠だと考えられており、新郎と新婦の権利と責任の概要を示している[1]。イスラームにおける離婚は様々な形をとることができ、個�...

Confessions of a Dangerous MindPoster rilis layar lebarSutradaraGeorge ClooneyProduserAndrew LazarSkenarioCharlie KaufmanBerdasarkanConfessions of a Dangerous Mindoleh Chuck BarrisPemeranDrew BarrymoreGeorge ClooneyJulia RobertsSam RockwellPenata musikAlex WurmanSinematograferNewton Thomas SigelPenyuntingStephen MirrionePerusahaanproduksiMad Chance DistributorMiramaxTanggal rilis 31 Desember 2002 (2002-12-31) (Perilisan terbatas) 24 Januari 2003 (2003-01-24) Durasi113 meni...

 

Earthquake in South Carolina, United States 1886 Charleston earthquakeDamage on Tradd StreetShow map of South CarolinaShow map of the United StatesUTC time1886-09-01 02:51:00USGS-ANSSComCatLocal dateAugust 31, 1886 (1886-08-31)Local time21:51Magnitude6.9–7.3 Mw[1]Epicenter32°54′N 80°00′W / 32.9°N 80.0°W / 32.9; -80.0[2]TypeUnknownAreas affectedSouth Carolina, United StatesTotal damage$5–6 million[2]Max. intensityM...

 

密西西比州 哥伦布城市綽號:Possum Town哥伦布位于密西西比州的位置坐标:33°30′06″N 88°24′54″W / 33.501666666667°N 88.415°W / 33.501666666667; -88.415国家 美國州密西西比州县朗兹县始建于1821年政府 • 市长罗伯特·史密斯 (民主党)面积 • 总计22.3 平方英里(57.8 平方公里) • 陸地21.4 平方英里(55.5 平方公里) • ...

 本表是動態列表,或許永遠不會完結。歡迎您參考可靠來源來查漏補缺。 潛伏於中華民國國軍中的中共間諜列表收錄根據公開資料來源,曾潛伏於中華民國國軍、被中國共產黨聲稱或承認,或者遭中華民國政府調查審判,為中華人民共和國和中國人民解放軍進行間諜行為的人物。以下列表以現今可查知時間為準,正確的間諜活動或洩漏機密時間可能早於或晚於以下所歸�...

 

Political proposal Not to be confused with Convention to propose amendments to the United States Constitution. This article is part of a series on thePolitics of the United States Federal government Constitution of the United States Law Taxation Policy Legislature United States Congress House of Representatives Speaker Mike Johnson (R) Majority Leader Steve Scalise (R) Minority Leader Hakeem Jeffries (D) Congressional districts (list) Non-voting members Senate President Kamala Harris (D) Pres...

 

Sharp edge formed by the intersection of two surfaces This article is about the architectural element. For the company, see Arris International. For other uses, see Arris (disambiguation). This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Arris – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (February 2023) (Lea...

American manufacturing company Workhorse GroupFormerlyAMP Electric Vehicles (2007–2015)Company typePublicTraded asNasdaq: WKHSIndustryTransportation, automotive, aerospaceFounded2007 (2007)FounderStephen BurnsHeadquartersSharonville, Ohio, United StatesNumber of locations4Area servedNorth AmericaKey peopleRichard Dauch (CEO)Number of employees331Websiteworkhorse.com Workhorse Group Incorporated, originally AMP Electric Vehicles, is an original equipment manufacturer and technology...

 

Art from Czech culture Part of a series on theCulture of the Czech Republic Architecture Art Cinema Cuisine Folklore Literature Music Mythology Philosophy Sports Theatre Czech Republic portalvte Copy of Venus of Petřkovice beside that of Venus of Dolní Věstonice at an exhibition in the National Museum, Prague International Gothic bust of the Virgin, 1390-1395, painted terracotta Czech art is the visual and plastic arts that have been created in the Czech Republic and the various states tha...

 

← липень → Пн Вт Ср Чт Пт Сб Нд 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31         2024 рік 19 липня — 200-й день року (201-й у високосні роки) в григоріанському календарі. До кінця року залишається 165 днів. Цей день в історії: 18 липня—19 липня—20 липня Зміст 1 Свята...

Island in Highland, Scotland This article is about the island. For the Caledonian MacBrayne (Calmac) ferry, see MV Raasay. RaasayScottish Gaelic nameRatharsairPronunciation[ˈrˠa.arˠs̪əɾʲ] ⓘOld Norse nameRaa-s-oy or Ross-oy, Rásey or HrosseyMeaning of nameOld Norse for roe deer island or possibly horse islandDùn Caan from Loch na MnaLocationRaasayRaasay shown within the Skye areaOS grid referenceNG579395Coordinates57°24′N 6°02′W / 57.400°N 6.033°W...

 

マジンボーン ジャンル バトル、変身ヒーロー、アクション ゲーム 対応機種 データカードダス 開発元 ディンプス 発売元 バンダイ キャラクターデザイン 村田雄介、石川ヒデキ(CG) 稼動時期 2014年4月24日 - 2015年8月31日 アニメ 原作 東映アニメーション シリーズディレクター 宇田鋼之介 シリーズ構成 羽原大介(第1話 - 第6話)、村山功 キャラクターデザイン 大西亮...