Cooperative breeding

Cooperative breeding is a social system characterized by alloparental care: offspring receive care not only from their parents, but also from additional group members, often called helpers.[1] Cooperative breeding encompasses a wide variety of group structures, from a breeding pair with helpers that are offspring from a previous season,[2] to groups with multiple breeding males and females (polygynandry) and helpers that are the adult offspring of some but not all of the breeders in the group,[3] to groups in which helpers sometimes achieve co-breeding status by producing their own offspring as part of the group's brood.[4] Cooperative breeding occurs across taxonomic groups including birds,[5] mammals,[6] fish,[7] and insects.[8]

Costs for helpers include a fitness reduction, increased territory defense, offspring guarding and an increased cost of growth. Benefits for helpers include a reduced chance of predation, increased foraging time, territory inheritance, increased environmental conditions and an inclusive fitness. Inclusive fitness is the sum of all direct and indirect fitness, where direct fitness is defined as the amount of fitness gained through producing offspring. Indirect fitness is defined as the amount of fitness gained through aiding the offspring of related individuals, that is, relatives are able to indirectly pass on their genes through increasing the fitness of related offspring.[9] This is also called kin selection.[10]

For the breeding pair, costs include increased mate guarding and suppression of subordinate mating. Breeders receive benefits as reductions in offspring care and territory maintenance. Their primary benefit is an increased reproductive rate and survival.

Cooperative breeding causes the reproductive success of all sexually mature adults to be skewed towards one mating pair. This means the reproductive fitness of the group is held within a select few breeding members and helpers have little to no reproductive fitness.[11] With this system, breeders gain an increased reproductive fitness, while helpers gain an increased inclusive fitness.[11]

Evolution

Many hypotheses have been presented to explain the evolution of cooperative breeding. The concept behind cooperative breeding is the forfeiting of an individual's reproductive fitness to aid the reproductive success of others. This concept is hard to understand and the evolution of cooperative breeding is important, but difficult to explain. Most hypotheses aim to determine the reason helpers selectively reduce their fitness and take on an alloparental role.

Kin selection is the evolutionary strategy of aiding the reproductive success of related organisms, even at a cost to the own individual's direct fitness. Hamilton's rule (rB−C>0) explains that kin selection will exist if the genetic relatedness (r) of the aided recipient to the aiding individual, times the benefit to the aid recipient (B) is greater than the cost to the aiding individual (C).[9] For example, the chestnut-crowned babbler (Pomatostomus ruficeps) has been found to have high rates of kin selection. Helpers are predominantly found aiding closely related broods over nonrelated broods.[12] Additional species such as Neolamprologus pulcher have shown that kin selection is a dominant driving force for cooperative breeding.[12]

Group augmentation presents a second hypothesis towards the evolution of cooperative breeding. This hypothesis suggests that increasing the size of the group through the addition of helpers aids in individual survival and may increase the helper's future breeding success.[13] Group augmentation is favored if the grouping provides passive benefits for helpers in addition to inclusive fitness.[14] By group augmenting, each individual member reduces their chances of becoming a victim of predation. Additionally, an increase in members reduces each helper's duration as a sentinel (standing upon a high surface to survey for predators) or babysitting (guarding the offspring and den). The reduction in these guarding behaviors enables helpers to forage for longer periods.[15]

Lukas et al. proposed an evolutionary model for cooperative breeding, which linked the coevolution of polytocy, production of multiple offspring, and monotocy, production of single offspring, with the evolution of cooperative breeding. The model is based on the evolution of larger litters forcing the need for helpers to maintain the high reproductive costs, thus leading to cooperative breeding. Lukas et al. suggests polytocy may have encouraged the evolution of cooperative breeding. Their proposed model suggests the transition from monotocy to polytocy is favorable. Additionally, they found the transition from polytocy without cooperative breeding to polytocy with cooperative breeding is highly favorable. This suggests cooperative breeding evolved from noncooperative breeding monotocy to cooperative breeding polytocy.[1]

Today, there is growing support for the theory that cooperative breeding evolved by means of some form of mutualism or reciprocity. Mutualism is a form of symbiosis that is beneficial to both involved organisms. Mutualism has many forms and can occur when the benefits are immediate or deferred, when individuals exchange beneficial behaviors in turn, or when a group of individuals contribute to a common good, where it may be advantageous for all group members to help raise young. When a group raises young together, it may be advantageous because it maintains or increases the size of the group.[16] The greatest amount of research has been invested in reciprocal exchanges of beneficial behavior through the iterated prisoner's dilemma. In this model, two partners can either cooperate and exchange beneficial behavior or they can defect and refuse to help the other individual.[16]

Environmental conditions

Environmental conditions govern whether offspring disperse from their natal group or remain as helpers. Food or territory availability can encourage individuals to disperse and establish new breeding territories, but unfavorable conditions promote offspring to remain at the natal territory and become helpers to obtain an inclusive fitness.[17] Additionally, remaining at the natal territory enables offspring to possibly inherit the breeding role and/or territory of their parents.[18]

A final factor influencing cooperative breeding is sexual dispersal. Sexual dispersal is the movement of one sex, male or female, from the natal territory to establish new breeding grounds. This is highly regulated by the reproductive costs in producing a male versus a female offspring. Maternal investment within female offspring may be considerably higher than male offspring for one species, or vice versa for another. During unfavorable conditions the cheaper sex will be produced at higher ratios.[19]

A second factor affecting the sexual dispersal is the difference in ability of each sex to establish a new breeding territory. Carrion crow (Corvus corone) were found to produce more female offspring in favorable environmental conditions. Female Corvus corone have been found to establish successful breeding territories at a higher rate than males. Male Corvus corone were produced at a higher rate under unfavorable conditions. Males were found to remain at the natal territory and become helpers.[20] Thus, if environmental conditions favor the dispersal of a specific sex it is considered the dispersal sex. If environmental conditions are unfavorable females may produce the philopatric sex, therefore generating more helpers and increasing the occurrence of cooperative breeding.[20]

Costs

Breeders

Breeder costs consist of prenatal care, postnatal care and maintenance of breeding status. Prenatal care is the amount of maternal investment during fetus gestation and postnatal care is the investment following birth. Examples of prenatal care are fetal, placentae, uterus and mammary tissue development. Postnatal examples are lactation, food provisions and guarding behavior.[19]

Dominant males and females exhibit suppressive behaviors towards subordinates to maintain their breeding status. These suppressive acts are dependent upon the sex ratio of helpers. Therefore, the costs will be altered depending upon the helpers. For example, if there are more male helpers as compared to females, then the dominant male will suppress subordinate males and experience a higher cost. The opposite is true for females. Breeders will even suppress subordinates from mating with other subordinates.[21]

Helpers

The cost to helpers varies depending upon presence or absence of related offspring. The presence of offspring has been found to increase the helper's cost by the helper contributing to guard behaviors.[22] Guarding behaviors, such as babysitting, can cause individuals to experience weight loss on an exponential scale depending upon the duration of the activity. Other activities, such as sentinel behavior and bipedal surveillance, cause helpers to have reduced foraging intervals inhibiting their weight gains. The reduced foraging behavior and increased weight loss reduces their chance to breed successfully, but increases their inclusive fitness by increasing the survival of related offspring.[11][23][24]

Helpers contribute depending upon the cost. The act of helping requires an allocation of energy towards actually performing the behavior. Prolonged allocation of energy may greatly impact a helper's growth.[24] In banded mongoose (Mungos mungo) juvenile male helpers contribute far less than females. This is due to a difference in the age of sexual maturity.[24] Female banded mongooses reach sexual maturity at one year of age, but males reach sexual maturity at two years of age. The difference in age causes the prolonged energy allocation to be detrimental to a specific sex.[24]

Male juvenile Mungos mungo may reduce helping behaviors until sexual maturity is reached. Similarly, if there is a lack of food due to environmental conditions, such as reduced rainfall, the degree of helper input may be reduced greatly within juveniles. Adults may maintain their full activity because they are sexually mature.[18]

Additionally, the costs of being a helper can be more detrimental to one sex. For example, territorial defense costs are generally male dependent and lactation is female dependent. Meerkats (Suricata suricatta) have exhibited male territory defense strategies, where male helpers will fend off intruding males to prevent such intruders from mating with subordinates or dominant females.[25] Additionally, subordinate female pregnant helpers are sometimes exiled from the group by a dominant female. This eviction causes the subordinate female to have an abortion, which frees up resources such as lactation and energy that can be used to help the dominant female and her pups.[10]

Rarely, a female helper or breeder will defend the territory while males are present. This suggests specific helping costs, such as territory defense, is rooted to one sex.[13]

Benefits

Breeders

Cooperative breeding reduces the costs of many maternal investments for breeding members. Helpers aid the breeding females with provisioning, lactation stress, guarding of offspring and prenatal investment.[13][19][26] Increasing the number of helpers enables a breeding female or male to maintain a healthier physique, higher fitness, increased lifespan and brood size.[22][27]

Female helpers can aid in lactation, but all helpers, male or female, can aid in food provisioning.[18][19] Helper food provisioning reduces the need for the dominant breeding pair to return to the den, thus allowing them to forage for longer periods. The dominant female and male will adjust their care input, or food provisioning, depending on the degree of activity of the helpers.[18]

The presence of helpers allows the breeding female to reduce her prenatal investment in the offspring, which may lead to altricial births; altricial is the production of young which are dependent upon adult aid to survive. This enables the breeding female to retain energy to be used within a new breeding attempt.[19] Overall, the addition of helpers to a breeding pair encourages multiple reproductions per year, and increases the rate of successful reproduction.[27]

Male breeders can benefit directly from reproducing with subordinate females and aiding in raising the young. This allows the male to obtain a “repayment investment” within these subordinate offspring. These offspring have a higher chance to become helpers once sexual maturity is reached. Thus, paying into their care will increase the dominant male's overall fitness in the future. This act ensures the dominant male subordinate helpers for future reproduction.[28]

Helpers

Helpers primarily benefit from an inclusive fitness.[1][17][23] Helpers maintain an inclusive fitness while aiding related breeders and offspring.[11] This type of kinship may lead to inheritance of quality foraging and breeding territories, which will increase the future fitness of helpers.[29] Additional, helpers experience an increased chance of being helped if they were once a helper.[27]

Helpers may also benefit from group interactions, such as huddling for thermodynamic benefits. These interactions provide necessary elements to survive.[15][29] They may also benefit from the increased group interaction on the level of cognitive concern for one another increasing their overall life span and survival.[30]

Finally, helpers may derive inclusive fitness benefits from influencing the extra-pair behaviour of their parents.[31] For example, by preventing their mothers from engaging in extra-pair matings, they can help their biological fathers protect their paternity and so increase their relatedness to future members of the cooperatively breeding group.[31]

Biological examples

Birds

Approximately eight percent of bird species are known to regularly engage in cooperative breeding, mainly among the Coraciiformes, Piciformes, basal Passeri and Sylvioidea.[32] Only a small fraction of these, for instance the Australian mudnesters, Australo-Papuan babblers and ground hornbills, are however absolutely obligately cooperative and cannot fledge young without helpers.[33]

The benefits of cooperative breeding in birds have been well-documented. One example is the azure-winged magpie (Cyanopica cyanus), in which studies found that the offspring's cell-mediated immune response was positively correlated with increase in the number of helpers at the nest.[34] Studies on cooperative breeding in birds have also shown that high levels of cooperative breeding are strongly associated with low annual adult mortality and small clutch sizes, though it remains unclear whether cooperative breeding is a cause or consequence.[35] It was originally suggested that cooperative breeding developed among bird species with low mortality rates as a consequence of “overcrowding” and thus fewer opportunities to claim territory and breed. However, many observers today believe cooperative breeding arose because of the need for helpers to rear young in the extremely infertile and unpredictable environments[36] of Australia and sub-Saharan Africa under the rare favourable conditions.[32]

Mammals

Across all mammalian species, less than 1% exhibit cooperative breeding strategies.[37] Phylogenetic analysis shows evidence of fourteen discrete evolutionary transitions to cooperative breeding within the class Mammalia.[38] These lineages are nine genera of rodents (Cryptomys, Heterocephalus, Microtus, Meriones, Rhabdomys, Castor, Atherurus and two in Peromyscus), four genera in Carnivora (Alopex, Canis, Lycaon, and in mongooses), and one genus of primates (Callitrichidae).[38] Cooperative breeding in mammals is not limited to these stated lineages, rather they are significant evolutionary events that provide the framework for understanding the origins and evolutionary pressures of cooperative breeding. All of these evolutionary transitions have occurred in lineages that had a socially monogamous or solitary breeding system, suggesting that strong kinship ties are an essential factor in the evolutionary history of cooperative breeding.[1][38]  Additionally, polytocy, or the birth of multiple offspring per birthing episode, is a highly correlated evolutionary determinant of cooperative breeding in mammals.[37]  These two factors, social monogamy and polytocy, are not evolutionary associated, suggesting that they are independent mechanisms leading to the evolution of cooperative breeding in mammals.[1] The global distribution of mammals with cooperative breeding systems is widespread across various climatic regions, but evidence shows that the initial transitions to cooperative breeding are associated to species in regions of high aridity.[37]

Meerkats

An older female watches over pups while alpha female is away.

Meerkats become reproductively active at one year of age and can have up to four litters per year. However, usually it is the alpha pair that reserves the right to mate and will usually kill any young that is not their own. While the alpha female is away from the group, females that have never reproduced lactate and hunt in order to feed the pups, as well as watch, protect, and defend them from predators. Although it was previously thought that a meerkat's contribution to a pup's diet depended on the degree of relatedness, it has been found that helpers vary in the number of food items they give to pups. This variation in food offering is due to variation in foraging success, sex, and age.[39] Research has additionally found that the level of help is not correlated to the kinship of the litters they are rearing.[40]

Canids

Cooperative breeding has been described in several canid species[41] including red wolves,[42] Arctic foxes[43] and Ethiopian wolves.[44]

Cooperative breeding increases the rate of reproduction in females and decreases the litter size.[37]

Primates

Cooperative breeding entails one or more individuals, usually females, acting as "helpers" to one or a few dominant female breeders, usually helpers' kin. This sociosexual system is rare in primates, so far demonstrated among Neotropical callitricids, including marmosets and tamarins.[45] Cooperative breeding requires "repression" of helpers' reproduction, by pheromones emitted by a breeder, by coercion, or by self-restraint. Sarah Blaffer Hrdy believes that cooperative breeding is an ancestral trait in humans, a controversial proposition.[citation needed] In most non-human primates, the reproductive success and survival of offspring is highly dependent to the mother's ability to produce food resources.[46]  Therefore, one component of cooperative breeding is the delegation of offspring holding, which allows the mother to forage without the added costs of holding her offspring.[46] Additionally, in primate species with cooperative breeding systems, females have shorter interbirth intervals. Female grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) form social groups and cooperatively breed with closely related female kin. The females benefit from sharing limited nesting spaces and increased nest defense but do not exhibit food provisioning behaviors as they are solitary foragers.[47]

Humans

Direct expression of cooperative breeding includes facultative parental care, including alloparenting, and extended post-menopausal lifespan in females, which forms the basis of the Grandmother Hypothesis.[48] Cooperative breeding in humans is theorized as the optimal solution to high energetic costs of survival due to nature of human diet, which involved high-quality foods often in need of processing and cooking.[49] Additionally, food provisioning in cooperate breeding societies may explain the relatively short period of weaning in humans, typically two to three years, when compared to non-human apes who wean their offspring for upwards of six years.[49]

Human offspring do not fall neatly into the dichotomous categorization of precocial versus altricial, and instead Portmann proposes they are "secondarily altricial" at birth due to the underdevelopment of neurological and cognitive capabilities.[50] Therefore, human offspring are highly dependent on caregiver investment, a necessity that serves as the precursor for theories on the development of pair-bonding, alloparenting, and cooperative breeding. The evolution of cooperative breeding in early Homo species also promoted other pro-social behaviors such as social learning, increased social tolerance, and shared intentionality especially in food acquisition.[51] Additionally, pro-social behaviors in cooperative breeding in humans had a by-product effect of enhancing cognitive capabilities, especially in social tasks involving coordination.[48]

Human mothers tend to have overlapping, dependent offspring due to shorter interbirth intervals, high fertility rates, and low infant mortality rates, thus imposing high energetic costs.[46] Unlike other species with cooperative breeding systems, human female "helpers" do not incur the cost of reproductive suppression at the benefit of a single, dominant breeding mother.[46] Instead, cooperative breeding is highly prevalent among grandparents, and juveniles, who are generally not competing for mating opportunities.[46] This intergenerational flow of resources supports the theory of mutualism as an evolutionary pathway to cooperative breeding in humans.[46]

References

  1. ^ a b c d e Lukas, D.; Clutton-Brock, T. (2012). "Life histories and the evolution of cooperative breeding in mammals". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 279 (1744): 4065–70. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1433. PMC 3427589. PMID 22874752.
  2. ^ Dickinson, Janis L.; Koenig, Walter D.; Pitelka, Frank A. (1996-06-20). "Fitness consequences of helping behavior in the western bluebird". Behavioral Ecology. 7 (2): 168–177. doi:10.1093/beheco/7.2.168. ISSN 1045-2249.
  3. ^ Haydock, J.; Koenig, W. D.; Stanback, M. T. (2001-06-01). "Shared parentage and incest avoidance in the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker". Molecular Ecology. 10 (6): 1515–1525. Bibcode:2001MolEc..10.1515H. doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01286.x. ISSN 1365-294X. PMID 11412372. S2CID 21904045.
  4. ^ Richardson, David S.; Burke, Terry; Komdeur, Jan; Dunn, P. (2002-11-01). "Direct benefits and the evolution of female-biased cooperative breeding in seychelles warblers". Evolution. 56 (11): 2313–2321. doi:10.1554/0014-3820(2002)056[2313:DBATEO]2.0.CO;2. ISSN 0014-3820. PMID 12487360. S2CID 198157808.
  5. ^ Cockburn, Andrew (1998-01-01). "Evolution of Helping Behavior in Cooperatively Breeding Birds". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 29: 141–177. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.29.1.141. JSTOR 221705.
  6. ^ Jennions, M (1994-01-01). "Cooperative breeding in mammals". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 9 (3): 89–93. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(94)90202-x. PMID 21236784.
  7. ^ Wong, Marian; Balshine, Sigal (2011-05-01). "The evolution of cooperative breeding in the African cichlid fish, Neolamprologus pulcher". Biological Reviews. 86 (2): 511–530. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00158.x. ISSN 1469-185X. PMID 20849492. S2CID 39910620.
  8. ^ Bourke, Andrew F. G.; Heinze, Jurgen (1994-09-30). "The Ecology of Communal Breeding: The Case of Multiple-Queen Leptothoracine Ants". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 345 (1314): 359–372. Bibcode:1994RSPTB.345..359B. doi:10.1098/rstb.1994.0115. ISSN 0962-8436.
  9. ^ a b Nicholas B. Davies, John R. Krebs, S. A. W. An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology.pdf. 522 (2012).
  10. ^ a b West, Stuart (2007). "Evolutionary explanations for cooperation". Current Biology. 17 (16): R661 – R672. Bibcode:2007CBio...17.R661W. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.004. PMID 17714660. S2CID 14869430.
  11. ^ a b c d Gerlach, Gabriele; Bartmann, Susann (2002-05-01). "Reproductive skew, costs, and benefits of cooperative breeding in female wood mice (Apodemus sylvaticus)". Behavioral Ecology. 13 (3): 408–418. doi:10.1093/beheco/13.3.408.
  12. ^ a b Browning, L. E.; Patrick, S. C.; Rollins, L. A; Griffith, S. C.; Russell, A F. (2012). "Kin selection, not group augmentation, predicts helping in an obligate cooperatively breeding bird". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 279 (1743): 3861–9. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1080. PMC 3415917. PMID 22787025.
  13. ^ a b c Mares, R.; Young, A. J.; Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2012). "Individual contributions to territory defence in a cooperative breeder: weighing up the benefits and costs". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 279 (1744): 3989–95. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1071. PMC 3427572. PMID 22810429.
  14. ^ Kokko, H.; Johnstone, R. A. (2001). "The evolution of cooperative breeding through group augmentation". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 268 (1463): 187–196. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1349. PMC 1088590. PMID 11209890.
  15. ^ a b Hatchwell, B. J. (2009). "The evolution of cooperative breeding in birds: kinship, dispersal and life history". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 364 (1533): 3217–27. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0109. PMC 2781872. PMID 19805429.
  16. ^ a b Clutton-Brock, Tim (2002). "Breeding Together: Kin Selection and Mutualism in Cooperative Vertebrates". Science. 296 (5565): 69–72. Bibcode:2002Sci...296...69C. doi:10.1126/science.296.5565.69. PMID 11935014. S2CID 12254536.
  17. ^ a b Marino, J.; Sillero-Zubiri, C.; Johnson, P. J.; Macdonald, D. W. (2012). "Ecological bases of philopatry and cooperation in Ethiopian wolves". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 66 (7): 1005–1015. doi:10.1007/s00265-012-1348-x. S2CID 17233754.
  18. ^ a b c d Nichols, H. J.; et al. (2012). "Food availability shapes patterns of helping effort in a cooperative mongoose". Animal Behaviour. 83 (6): 1377–1385. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.005. S2CID 53146761.
  19. ^ a b c d e Sharp, S. P.; English, S.; Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2012). "Maternal investment during pregnancy in wild meerkats". Evolutionary Ecology. 27 (5): 1033–1044. doi:10.1007/s10682-012-9615-x. S2CID 15575678.
  20. ^ a b Canestrari, D.; Vila, M.; Marcos, J. M.; Baglione, V. (2012). "Cooperatively breeding carrion crows adjust offspring sex ratio according to group composition". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 66 (9): 1225–1235. doi:10.1007/s00265-012-1375-7. S2CID 14646037.
  21. ^ Mitchell, J. S.; Jutzeler, E.; Heg, D.; Taborsky, M. (2009). "Gender Differences in the Costs that Subordinate Group Members Impose on Dominant Males in a Cooperative Breeder". Ethology. 115 (12): 1162–1174. Bibcode:2009Ethol.115.1162M. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01705.x. S2CID 10968991.
  22. ^ a b Santema, P.; Clutton-brock, T. (2013). "Meerkat helpers increase sentinel behaviour and bipedal vigilance in the presence of pups". Animal Behaviour. 85 (3): 655–661. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.029. S2CID 53171632.
  23. ^ a b Brotherton, P. N. M.; Riain, J. M. O.; Manser, M.; Skinner, J. D. (2013). "Costs of Cooperative Behaviour in Suricatas (Suricata Suricatta)". Proc Biol Sci. 265 (1392): 185–190. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0281. PMC 1688874. PMID 9493405.
  24. ^ a b c d Hodge, S. J. (2007). "Counting the costs: the evolution of male-biased care in the cooperatively breeding banded mongoose". Animal Behaviour. 74 (4): 911–919. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.024. S2CID 53152204.
  25. ^ Pauw, A (2000). "Parental care in a polygynous group of bat-eared foxes, Otocyon megalotis ( Carnivora : Canidae )". African Zoology. 35: 139–145. doi:10.1080/15627020.2000.11407200. S2CID 85572501.
  26. ^ Nichols, H. J.; Amos, W.; Cant; Bell, M. B. V.; Hodge, S. J. (2010). "Top males gain high reproductive success by guarding more successful females in a cooperatively breeding mongoose". Animal Behaviour. 80 (4): 649–657. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.025. S2CID 53148678.
  27. ^ a b c Charmantier, A.; Keyser, A. J.; Promislow, D. E. L. (2007). "First evidence for heritable variation in cooperative breeding behaviour". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 274 (1619): 1757–61. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0012. PMC 2493572. PMID 17490945.
  28. ^ Liedtke, J.; Fromhage, L. (2012). "When should cuckolded males care for extra-pair offspring?". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 279 (1739): 2877–82. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2691. PMC 3367774. PMID 22438493.
  29. ^ a b Sorato, E.; Gullett, P. R.; Griffith, S. C.; Russell, A. F. (2012). "Effects of predation risk on foraging behaviour and group size: adaptations in a social cooperative species". Animal Behaviour. 84 (4): 823–834. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.003. S2CID 53155823.
  30. ^ Isler, K.; Van Schaik, C. P. (2012). "How Our Ancestors Broke through the Gray Ceiling" (PDF). Current Anthropology. 53: S453 – S465. doi:10.1086/667623. S2CID 83106627.
  31. ^ a b Welbergen, J. A.; Quadar, S. (2006). "Mother guarding: how offspring may influence the extra-pair behaviour of their parents". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 273 (1599): 2363–2368. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3591. PMC 1636085. PMID 16928639.
  32. ^ a b Jetz, Walter; Rubinstein, Dustin R. (2011). "Environmental Uncertainty and the Global Biogeography of Cooperative Breeding in Birds". Current Biology. 21 (1): 72–8. Bibcode:2011CBio...21...72J. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.075. PMID 21185192.
  33. ^ See Cockburn, Andrew; "Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds"
  34. ^ Valencia, Juliana; Elena Solis; Gabrielle Sorci; Carlos de la Cruz (2006). "Positive correlation between helpers at nest and nestling immune response in cooperative breeding bird". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 60 (3): 399–404. doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0179-z. S2CID 1898846.
  35. ^ Arnold, Kathryn E.; Ian P. F. Owens (7 May 1998). "Cooperative breeding in birds: a comparative test of the life history hypothesis". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 265 (1398): 739–745. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0355. PMC 1689041.
  36. ^ See McMahon T.A. and Finlayson, B.; Global Runoff: Continental Comparisons of Annual Flows and Peak Discharges. ISBN 3-923381-27-1
  37. ^ a b c d Lukas, Dieter; Clutton-Brock, Tim (2017). "Climate and the distribution of cooperative breeding in mammals". Royal Society Open Science. 4 (1): 160897. Bibcode:2017RSOS....460897L. doi:10.1098/rsos.160897. ISSN 2054-5703. PMC 5319355. PMID 28280589.
  38. ^ a b c Lukas, Dieter; Clutton-Brock, Tim (2012-06-07). "Cooperative breeding and monogamy in mammalian societies". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 279 (1736): 2151–2156. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2468. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 3321711. PMID 22279167.
  39. ^ Clutton-Brock, T.H (2000). "Individual Contributions to babysitting in a cooperative mongoose, Suricata suricatta". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 267 (1440): 301–5. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1000. PMC 1690529. PMID 10714885.
  40. ^ Clutton-Brock, T.H.; Brotherton, P.N.M.; O'Riain, M.J.; Griffin, A.S.; Gaynor, D.; Kansky, R.; Sharpe, L.; McIlrath, G.M. (2000). "Contributions to cooperative rearing in meerkats". Animal Behaviour. 61 (4): 705–710. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1631. S2CID 53181036.
  41. ^ Moehlman, Patricia D., and H. E. R. I. B. E. R. T. Hofer. "Cooperative breeding, reproductive suppression, and body mass in canids." Cooperative breeding in mammals (1997): 76-128.
  42. ^ Sparkman, Amanda M.; et al. (2010). "Direct fitness benefits of delayed dispersal in the cooperatively breeding red wolf (Canis rufus)". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (1): 199–205. doi:10.1093/beheco/arq194.
  43. ^ Kullberg, Cecilia; Angerbjörn, Anders (1992). "Social Behaviour and Cooperative Breeding in Arctic Foxes, Alopex lagopus (L.), in a Semi-natural Environment" (PDF). Ethology. 90 (4): 321–335. Bibcode:1992Ethol..90..321K. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1992.tb00843.x.
  44. ^ van Kesteren, Freya; et al. (2013). "The physiology of cooperative breeding in a rare social canid; sex, suppression and pseudopregnancy in female Ethiopian wolves" (PDF). Physiology & Behavior. 122: 39–45. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.016. PMID 23994497. S2CID 46671897.
  45. ^ Tardif, Suzette D. (1994). "Relative energetic cost of infant care in small-bodied neotropical primates and its relation to infant-care patterns". American Journal of Primatology. 34 (2): 133–143. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350340205. ISSN 1098-2345. PMID 31936968. S2CID 55324849.
  46. ^ a b c d e f Kramer, Karen L. (2010-10-21). "Cooperative Breeding and its Significance to the Demographic Success of Humans". Annual Review of Anthropology. 39 (1): 417–436. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105054. ISSN 0084-6570.
  47. ^ Eberle, Manfred; Kappeler, Peter M. (2006-08-01). "Family insurance: kin selection and cooperative breeding in a solitary primate (Microcebus murinus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 60 (4): 582–588. doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0203-3. ISSN 1432-0762. S2CID 22186719.
  48. ^ a b van Schaik, Carel P.; Burkart, Judith M. (2010), Kappeler, Peter M.; Silk, Joan (eds.), "Mind the Gap: Cooperative Breeding and the Evolution of Our Unique Features", Mind the Gap, Springer Berlin Heidelberg, pp. 477–496, doi:10.1007/978-3-642-02725-3_22, ISBN 978-3-642-02724-6
  49. ^ a b Kramer, Karen L. (2014). "Why What Juveniles Do Matters in the Evolution of Cooperative Breeding". Human Nature. 25 (1): 49–65. doi:10.1007/s12110-013-9189-5. ISSN 1045-6767. PMID 24430798. S2CID 23028482.
  50. ^ Dunsworth, Holly M.; Warrener, Anna G.; Deacon, Terrence; Ellison, Peter T.; Pontzer, Herman (2012-09-18). "Metabolic hypothesis for human altriciality". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (38): 15212–15216. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10915212D. doi:10.1073/pnas.1205282109. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3458333. PMID 22932870.
  51. ^ Isler, Karin; van Schaik, Carel P. (2012). "Allomaternal care, life history and brain size evolution in mammals" (PDF). Journal of Human Evolution. 63 (1): 52–63. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.03.009. PMID 22578648. S2CID 40160308.

Read other articles:

Direktorat JenderalBina Pembangunan Daerah Kementerian Dalam Negeri Republik IndonesiaGambaran umumDasar hukumPeraturan Presiden Nomor 11 Tahun 2015; Peraturan Menteri Dalam Negeri Nomor 43 Tahun 2015Susunan organisasiDirektur JenderalDr. Teguh Setyabudi, M.Pd.Sekretaris Direktorat JenderalSri Purwaningsih, SH, M.AP. DirekturPEIPDDrs. Nyoto Suwignyo, MM.[1]SUPD IIr. Edison Siagian, ME.SUPD IIIwan Kurniawan, ST, MM.[1]SUPD IIIR. Budiono Subambang, ST, MPM.SUPD IVIr. Zanariah, M...

 

Artikel ini membutuhkan rujukan tambahan agar kualitasnya dapat dipastikan. Mohon bantu kami mengembangkan artikel ini dengan cara menambahkan rujukan ke sumber tepercaya. Pernyataan tak bersumber bisa saja dipertentangkan dan dihapus.Cari sumber: Jilin – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR (July 2014)Jilin 吉林省Provinsi TiongkokTranskripsi Name • Tionghoa吉林省 (Kota Jílín Shěng) • SingkatanJL / �...

 

Stasiun Singojuruh Stasiun Singojuruh dipotret dari dalam kereta api Sri Tanjung, 2019LokasiSingojuruh, Singojuruh, Banyuwangi, Jawa TimurIndonesiaKoordinat8°18′23.490″S 114°14′26.513″E / 8.30652500°S 114.24069806°E / -8.30652500; 114.24069806Koordinat: 8°18′23.490″S 114°14′26.513″E / 8.30652500°S 114.24069806°E / -8.30652500; 114.24069806Ketinggian+160 mOperator Kereta Api IndonesiaDaerah Operasi IX Jember Letakkm 66+316...

Election in NevadaMain article: 2020 United States presidential election 2020 United States presidential election in Nevada ← 2016 November 3, 2020 2024 → Turnout78.22%   Nominee Joe Biden Donald Trump Party Democratic Republican Home state Delaware Florida Running mate Kamala Harris Mike Pence Electoral vote 6 0 Popular vote 703,486 669,890 Percentage 50.06% 47.67% County results Congressional district results Precinct results Biden   ...

 

Запрос «Землеподобная планета»[d] перенаправляется сюда. На эту тему нужно создать отдельную статью. Сравнительные размеры планет земной группы Планеты земно́й гру́ппы — четыре планеты Солнечной системы: Меркурий, Венера, Земля и Марс[1]. Расположены во внутрен�...

 

Katedral ThièsKatedral Santa AnnaPrancis: Cathédrale Sainte-Anne de ThièsKatedral ThièsLokasiThièsNegaraSenegalDenominasiGereja Katolik RomaArsitekturStatusKatedralStatus fungsionalAktifAdministrasiKeuskupanKeuskupan Thiès Katedral Thiès atau yang bernama resmi Katedral Santa Anna (Prancis: Cathédrale Sainte-Anne de Thiès) adalah sebuah gereja katedral Katolik yang terletak di Thiès, Senegal. Katedral ini merupakan pusat kedudukan dan takhta bagi Keuskupan Thiès.[1]...

SK-1Casio SK-1PembuatCasioTanggal perilisan1985–?SpesifikasiPolifoni4TimbreMonotimbralTipe penyintesisSamplingAtenuatorADSR, 13 gelombang envelopeMemori penyimpanan5 suara preset PCM3 preset suara tambahan1 suara rancangan pengguna (sintesis harmonik)1 slot sampel400-step sequencerEfekPortamentoVibratoSample loopInput/outputKibor32 tuts miniCasio SK-1 adalah kibor sampler kecil yang dibuat oleh Casio pada tahun 1985.[1][2] SK-1 memiliki 32 tuts piano berukuran kecil, polifon...

 

Pour les articles homonymes, voir frappe (homonymie). Pour la frappe au sens de l'orientation des faces d'une pièce, voir Axe (numismatique). La frappe désigne l'action de frapper, au moyen d'une presse, un flan entre deux coins pour obtenir une monnaie. Les technologies de frappe ont beaucoup évolué afin de s'adapter aux besoins croissants de la circulation monétaire et aux exigences de qualité : Balancier français de 1831 La frappe au marteau : le monnayeur frappait au mar...

 

French botanist (1656–1708) Tourn. redirects here. For the medieval legal term, see Tourn. Joseph Pitton de TournefortJoseph Pitton de TournefortBorn5 June 1656Aix-en-Provence, Kingdom of FranceDied28 December 1708 (aged 52)Paris, Kingdom of FranceNationalityFrenchAlma materUniversity of ParisKnown forgenusScientific careerFieldsbotany Joseph Pitton de Tournefort (5 June 1656 – 28 December 1708) was a French botanist, notable as the first to make a clear definition ...

Pour les articles homonymes, voir Profil. Profil Le groupe Profil (Martine Havet à gauche) lors des répétitions du Concours Eurovision de la chanson 1980 à La Haye.Informations générales Pays d'origine France Années actives 1980 Composition du groupe Membres Martine HavetMartine BauerFrancis RignaultJean-Claude CorbelJean-Pierre Izbinski modifier Profil est un ancien groupe de musique français, composé de Martine Havet, Martine Bauer, Francis Rignault, Jean-Claude Corbel et Jean-Pie...

 

Archaeological site in Indiana, US United States historic placeEllerbusch archeological site (12W56)U.S. National Register of Historic Places Overview of the site from the southeastShow map of IndianaShow map of the United StatesLocationAtop the bluff northwest of the junction of State Road 662 and Ellerbursch Rd., west of Newburgh, IndianaCoordinates37°57′8″N 87°25′32″W / 37.95222°N 87.42556°W / 37.95222; -87.42556Area0.8 acres (0.32 ha)NRHP refe...

 

ХристианствоБиблия Ветхий Завет Новый Завет Евангелие Десять заповедей Нагорная проповедь Апокрифы Бог, Троица Бог Отец Иисус Христос Святой Дух История христианства Апостолы Хронология христианства Раннее христианство Гностическое христианство Вселенские соборы Н...

English painter and sculptor (1830–1896) The Right HonourableThe Lord LeightonPRAFrederic Leighton, Self-portrait, 1880BornFrederic Leighton(1830-12-03)3 December 1830Scarborough, North Riding of Yorkshire, EnglandDied25 January 1896(1896-01-25) (aged 65)Kensington, London, EnglandEducationEduard von SteinleGiovanni CostaKnown forPainting and sculptureNotable workFlaming JuneMovementAcademicism, Pre-Raphaelite and British AestheticismAwardsPrix de RomeLégion d'honneurSignature Fr...

 

莎拉·阿什頓-西里洛2023年8月,阿什頓-西里洛穿著軍服出生 (1977-07-09) 1977年7月9日(46歲) 美國佛羅里達州国籍 美國别名莎拉·阿什頓(Sarah Ashton)莎拉·西里洛(Sarah Cirillo)金髮女郎(Blonde)职业記者、活動家、政治活動家和候選人、軍醫活跃时期2020年—雇主內華達州共和黨候選人(2020年)《Political.tips》(2020年—)《LGBTQ國度》(2022年3月—2022年10月)烏克蘭媒�...

 

Piercarlo Beretta (in piedi, a sinistra) nel 1952, in veste di commissario tecnico della nazionale italiana. Piercarlo Beretta detto Carlino (Gardone Val Trompia, 1908 – Gardone Val Trompia, 5 marzo 1984) è stato un imprenditore e dirigente sportivo italiano. Fu personaggio poliedrico, erede della più antica casa industriale del mondo, posseduta da 480 anni dalla sua famiglia, la Beretta. Biografia Carlino Beretta era un industriale di stampo antico, riservato e schivo di pubblicità, uno...

Place in Al Wakrah Municipality, QatarKhawr al Udayd خور العديدSand dunes at Khawr al Udayd, photographed in 2004.Khawr al UdaydCoordinates: 24°37′48″N 51°17′46″E / 24.630°N 51.296°E / 24.630; 51.296Country QatarMunicipalityAl Wakrah MunicipalityArea • Total705 km2 (272 sq mi)Population (2015) • Total7 • Density0.0099/km2 (0.026/sq mi) Khawr al Udayd, (Arabic: خور العديد; a...

 

This article relies largely or entirely on a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.Find sources: Manila High School Intramuros – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2014) Secondary public school in PhilippinesManila High School(Mataas na Paaralan ng Maynila)External view of Manila High SchoolAddressVictoria-Muralia Street I...

 

Major goddess in Hinduism For other uses, see Parvati (disambiguation). Urvi redirects here. For another use, see Urvi (film). ParvatiMother Goddess[1]Goddess of Power, Nourishment, Devotion, Motherhood, Fertility, and Harmony[2]Supreme Goddess in ShaivismMember of Tridevi and Pancha PrakritiParvati with her son GaneshaOther namesUma, Gauri, Durga, Kali, Aparna, Girija, Haimavati, Shankari, Maheshvari, Ambika, Bhavani, ShivaniSanskrit transliterationPārvatīDevanagariपा�...

Sultan of Egypt and Syria (r. 1382–1389) (1390–1399) BarquqInside the Mosque-Madrasa of Sultan BarquqSultan of Egypt and SyriaFirst reign1382–1389PredecessorAs-Salih HajjiSuccessorAs-Salih HajjiSecond reign1390–1399PredecessorAs-Salih HajjiSuccessorAn-Nasir FarajBornc. 1336Kasa, Circassia[1]Died20 June 1399 (aged c. 63)Spouse Khawand Fatima Sitti Hajar Tandu Khatun Khawand Shirin Qunnuq-Bey Khawand Baraka Sul Khawand Hajj Malak IssueAn-Nasir FarajIzz ad-Din Abd al-AzizIbrahimK...

 

This article is about the ancient city in Lower Egypt. For the ancient city in Upper Egypt, see Athribis (Upper Egypt). City in Lower EgyptAthribisCityBlock relief usurped by Ramesses II, 19th DynastyAthribisCoordinates: 30°28′00″N 31°11′00″E / 30.46667°N 31.18333°E / 30.46667; 31.18333CountryLower Egypt ḥt tꜣ ḥrj jb[1][2]in hieroglyphs Athribis (Arabic: أتريب; Greek: Ἄθριβις,[3] from the original Ancient Egyptian:...