Schema of the proposed development of the axial twist. Developmental phases are (from top to bottom): (1) the embryo turns on its left side; (2) the anterior head grows in the same direction, but the rest of the body grows oppositely into a twist. So that ultimately (3) external bilateral symmetry is regained. Note that there is no evolutionary pressure for internal symmetry so the heart (and other organs) remain asymmetric.
The axial twist theory (a.k.a. axial twist hypothesis) is a scientific theory that has been proposed to explain a range of unusual aspects of the body plan of vertebrates (including humans).[1] It states that the rostral part of the head is "turned around" regarding the rest of the body.[2] This end-part consists of the face (eyes, nose, and mouth) as well as part of the brain (cerebrum and thalamus). According to the theory, the vertebrate body has a left-handed chirality.
The axial twist theory competes with a number of other proposals that focus on more limited, specific aspects, most of which explain contralateral forebrain organization, the phenomenon that the left side of the brain mainly controls the right side of the body and vice versa.[3] None of the proposed theories explaining this phenomenon, including axial twist theory, have gained general recognition.[4] The genetic basis underlying the proposed developmental twist is not yet understood.
The axial twist theory would explain various anatomical phenomena, and addresses how and when the proposed twist between the end of the head and the rest of the body develops. It also addresses the possible evolutionary history. One prediction of the theory was the aurofacial asymmetry, which was then found empirically,[5] albeit by one of the authors of the original theory.
According to the axial twist developmental model, the anterior part of the head turns against the rest of the body, except for the inner organs. Due to this twist, the forebrain and face are turned around such that left and right, but also anterior and posterior are flipped in the adult vertebrate.
History
In the end of the 19th century, the famous neuroscientist and Nobel Prize winner Santiago Ramón y Cajal proposed a theory to explain the contralateral organization of the forebrain that was rapidly and widely accepted.[6][4] This theory, the visual map theory, proposes that the optic chiasm restores the retinal image on the visual cortex,[7] Cajal's theory remained virtually undisputed for more than a century.[8][9] However, more ideas have been put forward (see section #Relation to other theories and hypotheses).
Embryology
Although the embryological development of the axial twist has not been studied explicitly, there are clear indications from the zebrafish and the chick.[1] The twist begins briefly after the neurulation and commences in a rostrocaudal (front-to-tail) direction.[10]
Philipp Keller's group traced each cell of developing zebrafish embryos until the first heartbeat.[11] Tracing the movements of the cells in the future eye region and the hind part of the head, revealed opposite movement directions, in accordance with an axial twist. Whereas the left-eye eye-region cells tendentially moved outwards and downwards (ventrally), those of the right eye region moved out- and upwards, as visualized by a time-lapse video.[12] On the other hand, the surface cells of the hind side of the head moved to the left, consistent with an axial twist.[1]
The chick development has been studied well. The development is usually described according to the Hamburger–Hamilton stages.[13] The twisting begins during stage 6 on the rostral side of the head region[14] and commences until stage 14 towards the heart region.[10] Whereas the anterior head region rotates with the right side moving in an upward direction and the left side downward, the heart region moves in the opposite direction. In the end, the chick is turned on its right side, whereas the heart, not taking part in the twisting, has landed on the left side of the body.[1]
The axial twist emerges through opposite asymmetric development. This can be observed as a wave moving across the embryo from anterior to posterior. It is now well established, that the Nodal signaling cascade and the right-to-left flow produced by ciliated cells in the primitive streak are central in setting up the asymmetric organization. Three aspects of this growth wave are:[1]
Nodal, FGF8, and shh as well as the motor protein Kif5c have been associated with the asymmetric growth of the anterior primitive node, although only Nodal seems to be expressed before the initiation of the asymmetry.[15][16] The Nodal gene and a zinc finger gene (cSnR) control the asymmetric development of the heart.[17] The right-side-turn of the body is tightly linked to the same genetic mechanisms.[17]
Developmental malformations
In holoprosencephaly, the hemispheres of the cerebrum (or part of it) are not aligned on the left and right sides but only on the frontal and occipital sides of the skull, and the head usually remains very small. According to the axial twist theory, this represents an extreme case of Yakovlevian torque,[18] and
may occur when the cerebrum does not turn during early embryology.[1]
Cephalopagus or janiceps twins are conjoined twins who are born with two faces, one on either side of the head. These twins have two brains and two spinal cords, but these are located on the left and the right side of the body.[19] According to the axial twist model, the two nervous systems could not turn due to the complex configuration of the body and therefore remained on either side.[1]
Evolution
The axial twist is thought to have evolved in a common ancestor of all vertebrates, but the mechanism remains speculative. However, twisting and asymmetric development are well known from other deuterostomes, such as Echinodermata, Cephalochordata, and Tunicata; as well as from gastropods.[1]
de Lussanet et al. posit two possible evolutionary origins: one, that an ancestral vertebrate turned to its left side during the transition from a free-swimming larva to a bottom-feeding adult stage, like modern-day flatfishes. The other hypothesis is that an ancestor of vertebrates was a bottom-feeder that turned to the left to move around, with this body position evolving to the orientation of the free-swimming last common ancestor. This second hypothesis would connect to the lifecycle of the closely related Cephalochordata, which have a mouth that is initially on the left side before moving to ventral position but lack an axial twist. Following the hypothesis of Dzik et al. (1995), de Lussanet suggests that if the second hypothesis is correct, then the first stage was the enigmatic Ediacaran fossil Dickinsonia, and the second stage is represented by the early CambrianYunnanozoon.[1]
Even the most distant clades of vertebrates – the agnathanlampreys and hagfish – possess an optic chiasm and contralateral brain organization,[20] as well as a left-sided heart and asymmetric bowels.[21] Also, every vertebrate has a contralateral organization of the forebrain.[20] Fossil skull impressions of early vertebrates from the Ordovician and later show the presence of an optic chiasm.[22]
Morphology
The axial twist takes place in the early embryo of a vertebrate. There is an evolutionary pressure for animals towards bilateral symmetry, due to sexual selection (better looks to potential mates) and functional selection (e.g., better locomotion). The evolutionary pressure decreases with better symmetry. Accordingly, the pressure decreases as a body part is less associated with the body surface (less sexual selection) and the locomotor system (less functional selection).[5] Consequently, the axial twist theory predicts that small, systematic asymmetries remain on the outside of the body and that these asymmetries are larger on the inside of the body.[1][23][5]
The forebrain (cerebrum and thalamus) predominantly represents the opposite side of the body (and the visual world). However, motor control usually requires information from both sides of the body, and so the contralateral representation is by no means absolute. Rather, almost every region of the brain connects to both sides of the body.[26]
The Yakovlevian torque[24] (a.k.a. "counterclockwise brain torque"[27] refers to an anatomical peculiarity of the normal brain. On average, the frontal lobes are asymmetric to the left (the right lobe appears slightly larger than the left), whereas the occipital lobe is asymmetric to the right; the central sulcus and temporal lobe of the right cortical hemisphere are further to the front than those on the left. Overall, these asymmetries are equivalent to a slight rotation of the cerebrum (i.e. torque). Such a rotation is exactly as predicted by the axial twist theory, given that the cerebrum is not a superficial structure.[citation needed]
The torque is also known as "occipital bending"[28] if it is more strongly expressed on the occipital side than on the frontal side.
The spine is slightly asymmetric. In healthy subjects, the thoracic vertebrae (vertebra T6-T12) were on average asymmetric, such that the mid-line points to the right (2.5° in T6).[25] Thus, the Yakovlevian torque and the spinal asymmetry are in opposite direction, just as predicted by the axial twist theory.[citation needed]
If the right forebrain represents predominantly the left body and the left forebrain the right body, there should be positions of major nerve crossings behind the forebrain.[citation needed]
In the visual system, the eyes and its muscles lie in front of the twist, while the sensory and motor centers lie behind the twist. Thus, the developing nerves are predicted to seek their insertion on the opposite side of their origin.[citation needed]
Four of the cranial nerves serve the eye directly: one sensory and three motor nerves. The optic nerve is sensory and crosses the midline in the optic chiasm. The oculomotor nerve, trochlear nerve, and abducens nerve are motor nerves that control one or more of the eye muscles. The oculomotor nerve crosses the midline before leaving the central nervous system. The trochlear nerve crosses the midline in a chiasma on the dorsal side and the abducens innervates an eye muscle on the same side.[29]: Fig. 17.8
In the light of the axial twist theory, this complicated pattern can be understood. The eyes, like the mouth and the nose originate from the anterior head region, i.e. in front of the twist. The only cranial nerve that originates from the forebrain is the olfactory nerve (see below). All other cranial nerves originate from regions of the central nervous system that lie behind the twist.[1]
The optic nerve inserts on the optic tectum of the midbrain. In tetrapods and bony fish it also branches off to the LGN of the thalamus in the forebrain, but not in other vertebrates such as sharks and skates). In sharks, the visual center in the cerebrum obtains its fibers from the optic tectum. On the way, these fibers cross the midline again so that each hemisphere of the cerebrum of sharks represents the eye on the same side.[30][31] Thus, the optic tract largely follows the prediction of the axial twist theory. The branch towards the LGN exists only in tetrapods[29] and is therefore generally understood as acquired later in the evolution of vertebrates and thus makes an exception.[citation needed]
The abducens nucleus is located in the pons. The abducens nerve innervates the lateral rectus muscle of the eye in most vertebrates, except lampreys and hagfishes.[21] It thus seems that the lateral rectus muscle evolved later and independently of the other eye muscles, and presents an exception to the axial twist model.[1]
The olfactory tracts run parallel to the optic tract but do not form a chiasm. Accordingly, each olfactory bulb connects to the same-side centers of the frontal cerebrum. This is entirely consistent with the axial twist theory because the nose is part of the anterior head region which twists along with the forebrain.[citation needed] Since the primary olfactory centers are at home in the cerebrum (olfaction is the only sense that originates in the cerebrum), each olfactory lobe is predicted to be represented by the cerebrum on the same side, which is indeed the case: whereas the olfactory tracts neighbour the optic tracts directly they do not chiasmate but insert on the ipsilateral side of the cerebrum (See figure).[citation needed]
The aurofacial asymmetry expresses the position of the face (eyes, nose, mouth) with respect to the plane perpendicular to the axis through the ears. As shown in the graph, the asymmetry decreases until the age of 13. Since the axial twist is located between the ears and the face, it is predicted that the face grows from the left to the midline, as is indeed the case.[5]
The inner organs of the trunk are the regions on the body that are least mechanically associated with locomotion and the external body, and so are predicted by the axial twist theory to be the most asymmetric regions of the body. Other bilaterally symmetric animals such as insects and annelids are bilaterally symmetric also on the inside. The asymmetric development of the heart is well-researched.[17][32]
The question of why the heart should have a left-sided orientation, has been topic of scientific research before the axial twist theory was published, but none of the hypotheses could stand critical testing.[33]
The asymmetric orientation and position of the gastrointestinal tract arises during development by rotation (see Development of the digestive system). The lateral positions of the digestive organs are on the same side in all vertebrates liver and gall bladder on the right side and the stomach, pancreas, spleen and the final loop of the colon on the left side.[34]
There are no other theories or hypotheses that explain the entire spectrum covered by the axial twist theory,[citation needed] but a number of theories and hypotheses address isolated aspects in various depths.
In 1822 the French zoologist Étienne Geoffroy Saint-Hilaire noted that the organization of dorsal and ventral structures in the crayfish (an arthropod) is opposite that of mammals, and he proposed that mammals and other vertebrates are turned upside down.[35][36] As explained above, Marcel Kinsbourne proposed that the body (soma) but not the anterior head is inverted (hence somatic twist hypothesis).[2]
There is molecular evidence for the inversion hypothesis in almost all groups of deuterostomes.[37][38]
Somatic twist hypothesis
Marcel Kinsbourne's somatic twist hypothesis is most closely related to the axial twist theory. Both theories were presented as an improvement to the dorsoventral inversion hypothesis by the early 19th-century naturalistÉtienne Geoffroy Saint-Hilaire[1][2] In contrast to the axial twist, the somatic twist does not address the developmental aspects. Also, it only explains the contralateral brain but not the orientation of the internal organs.[23]
Asymmetry of the spine
No hypotheses have been published for the asymmetry of the spine. There is no other theory to explain the left position of the heart or the asymmetric orientation of the bowels and related organs.[25][5]
Contralateral forebrain
Several hypotheses have proposed a function for (certain aspects of) the contralateral brain (i.e. the left forebrain representing mostly the right body and the right forebrain representing mostly the left body). Most prominent is Cajal's.[citation needed]
Cajal's visual map theory
The visual map theory by Santiago Ramón y Cajal proposes that the optic chiasm restores the retinal image on the visual cortex.[7][8] Even though the theory is still supported,[4] several studies have pointed out the serious flaws in the theory.[1][2][39] Most importantly, the loop of the optic radiation undoes the potential repair of the optic projection on the cortex, which is the central idea of the theory. Also, the theory does not hold for important groups of vertebrates such as sharks because their cerebrum has an ipsilateral visual representation but a contralateral somatosensory and motor representation.[30][31]
Other functional hypotheses
Functional[40][41] or topological[42] explanations fail because same-side connections are just as important as crossing ones.[43] Moreover, these explanations leave open why the brainstem and the cerebellum have a same-side organization of the body.[1]
Brain lateralization
The Yakovlevian torque is often thought to reflect the lateralization of specific functions in the brain.[24]
The parcellation theory proposes that an increasing brain size can conserve coincidental contralateral organization,[26] but does not explain the optic chiasm, nor that most vertebrates have a rather small forebrain, especially the early forms.[44]
Open questions
The axial twist theory is a novel scientific discipline and very few scientific papers have presently addressed it directly.[1][2][23][5] Although a considerable volume of research exists on the genetic and embryological mechanisms of asymmetric development, an open question is how the twist is initiated and how the inversion of the left-right and up-down axes in the anterior head region is established.[citation needed]
The embryology of the twisting has been addressed only rudimentarily in the chick and the zebrafish.[1] The differences in timing and mechanisms across the vertebrate clades are completely unknown.[citation needed]
The evolution of the axial twist is an open question. The founders of the axial twist idea (de Lussanet & Osse, and Kinsbourne) agree that the axial twist is universal in vertebrates and probably is a feature of all chordates.[1][2] Although the asymmetric development of other chordates such as the Lancelet has been studied in detail, no study has analysed this development in the light of the axial twist theory. Moreover, even other deuterostomes, i.e. the echinoderms (sea stars, sea lilies, etc.) show a marked asymmetric development and even an axial twist.[45] This twist has remarkable similarities to that in vertebrates, but no study has addressed this at present. Lastly, the asymmetric and twisted development is well known from gastropods and the relation to asymmetric development in vertebrates is an important question.[citation needed]
It has been proposed that problems in the axial twist development may play a central role in developmental malformations such as holoprosencephaly[1] and skoliosis[5] but these have not been looked into.[citation needed]
^Flechsig, Paul (1899). "Vorwort zu die Struktur des Chiasma optikum". Rev. Trim. Micrográfica (in German). 3. Verlag Joh. A. Barth: III–VI.
^ abRamón y Cajal, Santiago (1898). "Estructura del quiasma óptico y teoría general de los entrecruzamientos de las vías nerviosas. (Structure of the Chiasma opticum and general theory of the crossing of nerve tracks)" [Die Structur des Chiasma opticum nebst einer allgemeine Theorie der Kreuzung der Nervenbahnen (German, 1899, Verlag Joh. A. Barth)]. Rev. Trim. Micrográfica (in Spanish). 3: 15–65.
^ abLlinás, R.R. (2003). "The contribution of Santiago Ramón y Cajal to functional neuroscience". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4 (1): 77–80. doi:10.1038/nrn1011. PMID12511864. S2CID30442863.
^Vulliemoz, Serge; Raineteau, Olivier; Jabaudon, Denis (2005). "Reaching beyond the midline: Why are human brains cross wired?". Lancet Neurol. 4 (2): 87–99. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(05)00990-7. PMID15664541.
^ abLopez-Sanchez, Carmen; Garcia-Martinez, Virginio; Schoenwolf, Gary C. (2001). "Localization of cells of the prospective neural plate, heart and somites within the primitive streak and epiblast of avian embryos at intermediate primitive-streak stages". Cells Tissues Organs. 169 (4): 334–346. doi:10.1159/000047900. PMID11490112. S2CID238761.
^Tsikolia, N; Schröder, S; Schwartz, P; Viebahn, C. (2012). "Paraxial left-sided nodal expression and the start of left-right patterning in the early chick embryo". Differentiation. 84 (5): 380–391. doi:10.1016/j.diff.2012.09.001. PMID23142734.
^ abcIsaac, Alison; Sargent, Michael G.; Cooke, Jonathan (1997). "Control of Vertebrate Left-Right Asymmetry by a Snail-Related Zinc Finger Gene". Science. 275 (5304): 1301–1304. doi:10.1126/science.275.5304.1301. PMID9036854. S2CID19753550.
^ abcKouwenhoven, Jan-Willem; Vincken, Koen L.; Bartels, Lambertus W.; Castelein, Rene M. (2006). "Analysis of preexistent vertebral rotation in the normal spine". Spine. 31 (13): 1467–1472. doi:10.1097/01.brs.0000219938.14686.b3. PMID16741456. S2CID2401041.
^ abcEbbesson, Sven O. E. (1980). "The parcellation theory and its relation to interspecific variability in brain organization, evolutionary and ontogenetic development, and neuronal plasticity". Cell Tissue Res. 213 (2): 179–212. doi:10.1007/BF00234781. PMID7459999.
^ abNieuwenhuys, Rudolf; Voogd, Jan; van Huijzen, C. (2008). The human central nervous system (4th ed.). Berlin: Springer.
^ abLuiten, P. G. M. (1981). "Two visual pathways to the telencephalon in the nurse shark (Ginglymostoma cirratum). I. retinal projections". J. Comp. Neurol. 96 (4): 531–538. doi:10.1002/cne.901960402. PMID7204669. S2CID36572723.
^ abLuiten, P. G. M. (1981). "Two visual pathways to the telencephalon in the nurse shark (Ginglymostoma cirratum). II. ascending thalamo-telencephalic connections". J. Comp. Neurol. 96 (4): 539–548. doi:10.1002/cne.901960403. PMID7204670. S2CID13333558.
^Voronov, Dmitry A.; Alford, Patrick W.; Xu, Gang; Taber, Larry A. (2004). "The role of mechanical forces in dextral rotation during cardiac looping in the chick embryo". Developmental Biology. 272 (2): 339–350. doi:10.1016/j.ydbio.2004.04.033. PMID15282152.
^Westheide, Wilfried; Rieger, Reinhard M. (2004). Spezielle Zoologie, Teil 2: Wirbel- und Schädeltiere (in German). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. p. 712. ISBN9783827415752.
^Geoffroy-Saint-Hilaire, Étienne (1822). "Considérations générales sur la vertèbre". Mémoires du Muséum d'Histoire Naturelle (in French). 9: 89–119.
Chris Smith (11 Aug 2023). "Question of the Week". www.thenakedscientists.com (Podcast). Cambridge University. Event occurs at 23:30. Retrieved 14 Aug 2023.