Inferior view of the brain, depicting the cerebral lobes. Lesions on the occipito-temporal lobes are correlated with associative agnosia.
Associative visual agnosia is a form of visual agnosia. It is an impairment in recognition or assigning meaning to a stimulus that is accurately perceived and not associated with a generalized deficit in intelligence, memory, language or attention.[1] The disorder appears to be very uncommon in a "pure" or uncomplicated form and is usually accompanied by other complex neuropsychological problems due to the nature of the etiology.[1] Affected individuals can accurately distinguish the object, as demonstrated by the ability to draw a picture of it or categorize accurately, yet they are unable to identify the object, its features or its functions.
Classification
Agnosias are sensory modality specific, usually classified as visual, auditory, or tactile.[2][3] Associative visual agnosia refers to a subtype of visual agnosia, which was labeled by Lissauer (1890), as an inability to connect the visual percept (mental representation of something being perceived through the senses) with its related semantic information stored in memory, such as, its name, use, and description.[4][5][6] This is distinguished from the visual apperceptive form of visual agnosia, apperceptive visual agnosia, which is an inability to produce a complete percept, and is associated with a failure in higher order perceptual processing where feature integration is impaired, though individual features can be distinguished.[7] In reality, patients often fall between both distinctions, with some degree of perceptual disturbances exhibited in most cases, and in some cases, patients may be labeled as integrative agnostics when they fit the criteria for both forms.[1] Associative visual agnosias are often category-specific, where recognition of particular categories of items are differentially impaired, which can affect selective classes of stimuli, larger generalized groups or multiple intersecting categories. For example, deficits in recognizing stimuli can be as specific as familiar human faces or as diffuse as living things or non-living things.[7]
An agnosia that affects hearing, auditory sound agnosia, is broken into subdivisions based on level of processing impaired, and a semantic-associative form is investigated within the auditory agnosias.[2]
Most cases have injury to the occipital and temporal lobes and the critical site of injury appears to be in the left occipito-temporal region, often with involvement of the splenium of the corpus callosum.[11] The etiology of the cognitive impairment, as well the areas of the brain affected by lesions and stage of recovery are the primary determinants of the pattern of deficit.[1] More generalized recognition impairments, such as, animate object deficits, are associated with diffuse hypoxic damage, like carbon monoxide poisoning; more selective deficits are correlated with more isolated damage due to focal stroke.[1]
Damage to the left hemisphere of the brain has been explicitly implicated in the associative form of visual agnosia.[12][13] Goldberg suggested that the associative visual form of agnosia results from damage to the ventral stream of the brain, the occipito-temporal stream, which plays a key role in object recognition as the so-called "what" region of the brain, as opposed to the "where," dorsal stream.[12]
Theoretical explanations
Teuber[14] described the associative agnostic as having a "percept stripped of its meaning," because the affected individual cannot generate unique semantic information to identify the percept, since though it is fully formed, it fails to activate the semantic memory associated with the stimulus.[8] Warrington (1975)[15] offered that the problem lies in impaired access to generic engrams (memory traces) that describe categories of objects made up of a multitude of similar elements.[12] Essentially, damage to a modality-specific meaning process (semantic system), is proposed, either in terms of defective access to or a degradation of semantic memory store for visual semantic representations themselves.[13][16] The fact that agnosias are often restricted to impairments of particular types of stimuli, within distinct sensory modalities, suggests that there are separate modality specific pathways for the meaningful representation of objects and pictures, written material, familiar faces, and colors.[17]
Object recognition model
Cognitive psychology often conceptualizes this deficit as an impairment in the object recognition process. Currently visual agnosias are commonly explained in terms of cognitive models of object recognition or identification.[9] The cognitive system for visual object identification is a hierarchal process, broken up into multiple steps of processing.[16]
In the object recognition unit model by Marr (1980),[18] the process begins with sensory perception (vision) of the object, which results in an initial representation via feature extraction of basic forms and shapes. This is followed by an integration stage, where elements of the visual field combine to form a visual percept image, the 'primary sketch'. This is a 2+1⁄2 dimensional (2+1⁄2D) stage with a 'viewer-centered' object representation, where the features and qualities of the object are presented from the viewer's perspective.[5] The next stage is formation of a 3 dimensional (3D) 'object-centered' object representation, where the object's features and qualities are independent of any particular perspective. Impairment at this stage would be consistent with apperceptive agnosia.[1] This fully formed percept then triggers activation of stored structural object knowledge for familiar things.[9] This stage is referred to as "object recognition units" and distinctions between apperceptive and associative forms can be made based on presentation of a defect before or after this stage, respectively.[16] This is the level at which one is proposed to perceive familiarity toward an object, which activates the semantic memory system, containing meaning information for objects, as well as descriptive information about individual items and object classes. The semantic system can then trigger name retrieval for the objects. A patient who is not impaired up until the level of naming, retaining access to meaning information, are distinguished from agnostics and labeled as anomic.[1]
Non-abstracted view
In an alternate model of object recognition by Carbonnel et al.[19]episodic and semantic memory arise from the same memory traces, and no semantic representations are stored permanently in memory. By this view, the meaning of any stimulus emerges momentarily from reactivation of one's previous experiences with that entity. Each episode is made of several components of many different sensory modalities that are typically engaged during interactions with an object. In this scenario, a retrieval cue triggers reactivation of all episodic memory traces, in proportion to the similarity between the cue its 'echo,' the components shared by most activated traces. In a process called 're-injection,' the first echo acts as a further retrieval cue, evoking the 'second echo,' the less frequently associated components of the cue. Thus, the 're-injection' process provides a more complete meaning for the object. According to this model, different types of stimuli will evoke differential 'echos' based on typical interactions with them.[9] For example, a distinction is made between functional and visual components of various stimuli, such that impairment to these aspects of the memory trace will inhibit the re-injection process needed to complete the object representation. This theory has been used to explain category-specific agnosias that impair recognition of various object types, such as animals and words, to different degrees.[9]
Common forms of visual associative agnosia
Disorder
Recognition impairment
Retained abilities
Location of lesion
Visual object agnosia
Often specific to a particular category or categories of stimuli, i.e. living/animate things, tools, musical instruments, etc.
Images can be copied, demonstrating intact object perception
Objects usually identifiable by sensory modalities other than vision[20]
Can describe layouts of buildings or spaces, therefore topographical memory retained
Right posterior cingulate cortex
Visual object agnosia
Visual object agnosia (or semantic agnosia) is the most commonly encountered form of agnosia.[16] The clinical "definition" of the disorder is when an affected person is able to copy/draw things that they cannot recognize. Individuals often cannot identify, describe or mimic functions of items, though perception is intact, since images of objects can be copied or drawn.[7] Individuals may retain semantic knowledge of the items, as exemplified during tasks where objects are presented through alternate modalities, through touch or verbal naming or description. Some associative visual object agnostics retain the ability to categorize items by context or general category, though unable to name or describe them.[16] Diffuse hypoxic damage is the most common cause of visual object agnosias.[8]
Category-specific agnosia
Category-specific agnosias are differential impairments in subject knowledge or recognition abilities pertaining to specific classes of stimuli, such as living things vs. non-living things, animate vs. inanimate things, food, metals, musical instruments, etc. Some of the most common category-specific agnosias involve recognition impairments for living things, but not non-living things, or human faces, as in prosopagnosia. This type of deficit is typically associated with head injury or stroke, though other medical conditions have been implicated, such as, herpes encephalitis.[2]
Prosopagnosia
Prosopagnosia (or "face blindness") is a category-specific visual object agnosia, specifically, impairment in visual recognition of familiar faces, such as close friends, family, husbands, wives, and sometimes even their own faces. Individuals are often able to identify others through alternate characteristics, such as, voice, gait, context or unique facial features. This deficit is typically assessed through picture identification tasks of famous persons. This condition is associated with damage to the medial occipito-temporal gyri, including the fusiform and lingual gryi, as the suggested location of the brain's face recognition units.[7][11][16]
Associative and apperceptive forms
Two subtypes are distinguished behaviorally as being associative or apperceptive in nature. Associative prosopagnosia is characterized by an impairment in recognition of a familiar face as familiar; however, individuals retain the ability to distinguish between faces based on general features, such as, age, gender and emotional expression.[20] This subtype is distinguished through facial matching tasks or feature identification tasks of unknown faces.[16]
Pure alexia
Pure alexia ("alexia without dysgraphia" or "pure word blindness") is a category-specific agnosia, characterized by a distinct impairment in reading words, despite intact comprehension for verbally presented words, demonstrating retained semantic knowledge of words.[7][20] Perceptual abilities are also intact, as assessed by word-copying tasks.[2]
Cerebral achromatopsia
Cerebral achromatopsia, also known as Color agnosia, is a category-specific semantic impairment pertaining to semantic color associations, such that individuals retain perceptual abilities for distinguishing color, demonstrated through color categorization or hue perception tasks; however knowledge of typical color-object relationships is defective.[7][8][16] Color agnostics are assessed on performance coloring in black and white images of common items or identifying abnormally colored objects within a set of images.[2]
Overlap with color anomia
This deficit should be distinguished from color anomia, where semantic information about color is retained, but the name of a color cannot be retrieved, though co-occurrence is common. Both disorders linked to damage in the occipito-temporal cortex, especially in the left hemisphere, which is believed to play a significant role in color memory.[16]
Diagnosis
A recognition disorder is not considered to be agnosia unless there is a lack of aphasia, dementia, or other generalized defect that affects any stage of the object recognition process, such as, deficiencies in intelligence, linguistic ability, memory, attention or sensory perception.[1][2] Therefore, individuals must be assessed for language ability, auditory comprehension, fluency, repetition, praxis, and reading and writing.
Goals of clinical assessment of agnosia
Ruling out alternative conditions leading to the recognition impairment, such as, primary sensory disruption, dementia, aphasia, anomia, or unfamiliarity with the object category or elements.
Determination of the scope and specific nature of the recognition impairment. Including:
Specific sensory modality
Specific category of stimuli
Specific conditions under which recognition is possible[1]
Testing
Specialists, like ophthalmologists or audiologists, can test for perceptual abilities. Detailed testing is conducted, using specially formulated assessment materials, and referrals to neurological specialists is recommended to support a diagnosis via brain imaging or recording techniques. The separate stages of information processing in the object recognition model are often used to localize the processing level of the deficit.[1]
Testing usually consists of object identification and perception tasks including:
object-naming tasks
object categorization or figure matching
drawing or copying real objects or images or illustrations
unusual views tests
overlapping line drawings
partially degraded or fragmented image identification
Sensory modality testing allows practitioners to assess for generalized versus specific deficits, distinguishing visual agnosias from optic aphasia, which is a more generalized deficit in semantic knowledge for objects that spans multiple sensory modalities, indicating an impairment in the semantic representations themselves.[13]
Apperceptive vs. associative
The distinction between visual agnosias can be assessed based on the individual's ability to copy simple line drawings, figure contour tracking, and figure matching.[5] Apperceptive visual agnostics fail at these tasks, while associative visual agnostics are able to perform normally, though their copying of images or words is often slavish, lacking originality or personal interpretation.[1]
Treatment
The affected individual may not realize that they have a visual problem and may complain of becoming "clumsy" or "muddled" when performing familiar tasks such as setting the table or simple DIY.
Anosognosia, a lack of awareness of the deficit, is common and can cause therapeutic resistance.[2] In some agnosias, such as prosopagnosia, awareness of the deficit is often present; however shame and embarrassment regarding the symptoms can be a barrier in admission of a deficiency.[16] Because agnosias result from brain lesions, no direct treatment for them currently exists, and intervention is aimed at utilization of coping strategies by patients and those around them. Sensory compensation can also develop after one modality is impaired in agnostics[1]
General principles of treatment:
restitution
repetitive training of impaired ability
development of compensatory strategies utilizing retained cognitive functions[1]
Partial remediation is more likely in cases with traumatic/vascular lesions, where more focal damage occurs, than in cases where the deficit arises out of anoxic brain damage, which typically results in more diffuse damage and multiple cognitive impairments.[2] However, even with forms of compensation, some affected individuals may no longer be able to fulfill the requirements of their occupation or perform common tasks, such as, eating or navigating. Agnostics are likely to become more dependent on others and to experience significant changes to their lifestyle, which can lead to depression or adjustment disorders.[1]
References
^ abcdefghijklmnopBauer, Russell M. (2006). "The Agnosias". In Snyder, P.J. (ed.). Clinical Neuropsychology: A Pocket Handbook for Assessment (2nd Ed). P.D. Nussbaum, & D.L. Robins. American Psychological Association. pp. 508–533. ISBN978-1-59147-283-4. OCLC634761913.
^ abcdefghGhadiali, Eric (November–December 2004). "Agnosia"(PDF). ANCR. 4 (5). Retrieved 18 March 2012.
^Reed CL, Caselli RJ, Farah MJ (June 1996). "Tactile agnosia. Underlying impairment and implications for normal tactile object recognition". Brain. 119 (3): 875–88. doi:10.1093/brain/119.3.875. PMID8673499.
^Mack, James L.; Francois Boller (1977). "Associative visual agnosia and its related deficits: The role of the minor hemisphere in assigning meaning to visual perceptions". Neuropsychologia. 15 (2): 345–349. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(77)90044-6. PMID846640. S2CID23996895.
^Carbonnel, S.; A. Charnallet; D. David; J. Pellat (1997). "One or several semantic system(s): maybe none. Evidence from a case study of modality and category-specific "semantic" im- pairment". Cortex. 33 (3): 391–417. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70227-2. PMID9339326. S2CID4479759.
^ abcdFarah, Martha J., ed. (2000). "Chapter 12: Disorders". Patient-Based Approaches to Cognitive Neuroscience. Todd E Feinberg. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. pp. 79–84, 143–154. ISBN978-0262561235. Archived from the original on 2012-03-18. Retrieved 2012-04-10.
Chim cút ngực lamCoturnix chinensisTình trạng bảo tồnÍt quan tâm (IUCN 3.1)[1]Phân loại khoa họcGiới (regnum)AnimaliaNgành (phylum)ChordataLớp (class)AvesBộ (ordo)GalliformesHọ (familia)PhasianidaeChi (genus)CoturnixLoài (species)C. chinensisDanh pháp hai phầnCoturnix chinensisLinnaeus, 1766 Danh pháp đồng nghĩa Excalfactoria chinensis E. chinensis Coturnix chinensis là một loài chim trong họ Phasianidae.[2] Chú thích ^ BirdLife ...
Cet article est une ébauche concernant une chronologie ou une date et les Territoires du Nord-Ouest. Vous pouvez partager vos connaissances en l’améliorant (comment ?) selon les recommandations des projets correspondants. Chronologie des Territoires du Nord-Ouest 1867 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874 1875 ►► Chronologies Données clés 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874Décennies :1840 1850 1860 1870 1880 1890 1900Siècles :XVIIe XVIIIe X...
Stadion Balgarska ArmiaFull nameBulgarian Army StadiumFormer namesAtletik Park (1923–1944) Stadion Chavdar (1944–1948) Narodna Armia (1948–1990) Balgarska Armia (1990–)LocationSofia, BulgariaCoordinates42°41′3″N 23°20′23″E / 42.68417°N 23.33972°E / 42.68417; 23.33972OwnerMinistry of Youth and Sports (50%) CSKA Sofia (50%)OperatorCSKA SofiaCapacity22 995 (18 495 seats)[1][2]Field size105m x 68mSurfaceGrassConstructionBuilt1965Opened19...
Pemilihan Umum Bupati Lampung Tengah 2020201520249 Desember 2020[1]Kandidat Calon Loekman Djoyosoemarto Musa Ahmad Nessy Kalviya Partai PDI-P Partai Golongan Karya NasDem Pendamping Ilyas Hayani Muda Ardito Wijaya Imam Suhadi Suara rakyat 129.031 322.797 189.363 Persentase 20.1% 50.3% 29.5% Peta persebaran suara Peta Lampung yang menyoroti Kabupaten Lampung Tengah Bupati dan Wakil Bupati petahanaMustafa danLoekman Djoyosoemarto Partai Golongan Karya Bupati dan Wakil Bupati...
Nien Sön Ling um 1920 Nien Sön Ling (* 17. November 1892 in der Provinz Tschekiang, Kaiserreich China; † 22. Dezember 1946 in Berlin)[1] war ein chinesischer Schauspieler beim deutschen Film. Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Leben und Wirken 2 Filmografie (Auswahl) 3 Literatur 4 Weblinks 5 Einzelnachweise Leben und Wirken Über Lings Herkunft und frühes Leben ist derzeit nichts bekannt; es gibt Hinweise, dass er seit seiner Ankunft in Europa noch vor dem Ersten Weltkrieg als Kaufmann (Import...
هذه المقالة غير مكتملة، وربما تنقصها بعض المعلومات الضرورية. فضلًا ساعد في تطويرها بإضافة مزيدٍ من المعلومات. (يوليو 2017) الانفجار تحت الماء: 1. غيمة بخار الماء2. الماء3. الطبقة4. تفق الحمم5. قناة الحمم البركانية الذائبة6. غرفة الصّهر7. الخندق8. الحمم المتوسدة الثوران تحت الماء هو...
Korean-American model and beauty queen Emma BroylesBornEmma Leigh Broyles (2001-07-25) July 25, 2001 (age 22)Anchorage, Alaska, USEducationArizona State University (current)Known forFirst Miss Alaska to win the Miss America titleTitleMiss Anchorage's Outstanding Teen 2017Miss Alaska's Outstanding Teen 2017Distinguished Young Woman of Alaska 2019Miss Alaska 2021Miss America 2022TermDecember 16, 2021 – December 15, 2022PredecessorCamille SchrierSuccessorGrace Stanke Emma Leigh Broyl...
Tempestade tropical Fay Tempestade tropical (SSHWS/NWS) Imagem de satélite Trajetória Formação 9 de julho de 2020 Dissipação 11 de julho de 2020 (Extratropical depois de 11 de julho) Ventos mais fortes sustentado 1 min.: 95 km/h (60 mph) Pressão mais baixa 998 hPa (mbar); 29.47 inHg Fatalidades 6 total Danos ≥350 milhões(Perdas económicas totais) Áreas afectadas Nordeste dos Estados Unidos (Nova Inglaterra e Atlântico Médio dos Estados Unidos) Parte da Temporada de furacões...
Czech footballer Jakub Brabec Brabec with the Czech Republic in 2019Personal informationFull name Jakub BrabecDate of birth (1992-08-06) 6 August 1992 (age 31)Place of birth Prague, CzechoslovakiaHeight 1.86 m (6 ft 1 in)Position(s) Centre-backTeam informationCurrent team ArisNumber 14Youth career–2008 Sparta PragueSenior career*Years Team Apps (Gls)2008–2011 Viktoria Žižkov 22 (0)2011–2016 Sparta Prague 71 (6)2011–2012 → Sparta Prague II (loan) 18 (3)2012–20...
Function called at the end of an object's lifetime In object-oriented programming, a destructor (sometimes abbreviated dtor[1]) is a method which is invoked mechanically just before the memory of the object is released.[2] It can happen when its lifetime is bound to scope and the execution leaves the scope, when it is embedded in another object whose lifetime ends, or when it was allocated dynamically and is released explicitly. Its main purpose is to free the resources (memor...
Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Hutan Dipterokarp Bukit – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR Hutan Dipterokarp Bukit adalah kelas hutan yang terluas di Semenanjung Malaysia. Hutan ini berada di ketinggian antara 300-750 ...
Київська Академія мистецтвКАМ|день університету|почесний голова 50°30′04″ пн. ш. 30°30′57″ сх. д. / 50.501135000027773003° пн. ш. 30.51583500002777782° сх. д. / 50.501135000027773003; 30.51583500002777782Координати: 50°30′04″ пн. ш. 30°30′57″ сх. д. / 50.501135000027773003° пн....
Indian photographer and actor This article includes a list of general references, but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations. Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations. (March 2013) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Karan KapoorKapoor in January 2020Born (1962-01-18) 18 January 1962 (age 61)Mumbai, Maharashtra, IndiaOccupation(s)Actor, photographer, model, businessmanYears active1962-presentSpouseLorna KapoorChildren2Par...
Parte da série sobrePolítica de Israel Leis Básicas Lei do Retorno Lei de Jerusalém Presidente Presidente - Isaac Herzog Executivo Primeiro-Ministro - Naftali Bennett Primeiro-ministro Alternativo - Yair Lapid Governo Legislativo Knesset Líder da Oposição Judiciário Suprema Corte Conselheiro Jurídico Eleições Eleições parlamentares - 2020 2021 Partidos políticos Subdivisões Distritos Governo local Portal de Israelvde A Lei de Jerusalém é o nome comum da Lei Básica: Jerusalé...
1964 film The GorillasDirected byJean GiraultWritten byJacques Vilfrid Jean GiraultProduced byRaymond DanonStarringDarry CowlFrancis BlancheBernard DhéranCinematographyMarc FossardEdited byJean-Michel Gautier Music byRaymond LefevrePaul MauriatProductioncompaniesComacicoLes Films CopernicDistributed byComacicoRelease date23 December 1964Running time88 minutesCountryFranceLanguageFrench The Gorillas (French: Les gorilles) is a 1964 French comedy film directed by Jean Girault and starring Darr...
Railway station in Toride, Ibaraki Prefecture, Japan This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Inatoi Station – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (November 2020) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Inatoi Station稲戸井駅Inatoi Station, April 2008General informationLocatio...
Safe for storing firearms This article uses bare URLs, which are uninformative and vulnerable to link rot. Please consider converting them to full citations to ensure the article remains verifiable and maintains a consistent citation style. Several templates and tools are available to assist in formatting, such as reFill (documentation) and Citation bot (documentation). (August 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) This article needs additional citations for verification....
Ibn al-Banna redirects here. For the 11th-century author and diarist, see Abu Ali ibn al-Banna. Moroccan mathematician and astronomer Ibn al‐Bannāʾ al‐MarrākushīBorn29 or 30 December 1256Marrakech, Almohad Caliphate[1][2]Died31 July 1321Marrakech, Marinid SultanateAcademic backgroundInfluencesAl-Zarqali, Ibn Ishaq al-TunisiAcademic workEraIslamic Golden AgeMain interestsMathematics, astronomy Ibn al‐Bannāʾ al‐Marrākushī (Arabic: ابن البناء المراك...
1881 painting by Édouard Manet Dead Eagle OwlYear1881MediumOil on canvasDimensions97 cm × 64 cm (38 in × 25 in)LocationFoundation E.G. Buhrle, ZurichDead Eagle Owl (French: Le Grand-duc) is an 1881 oil-on-canvas painting by Édouard Manet. One of the very few hunting still lifes in Manet's oeuvre, it depicts a dead Eurasian eagle-owl hanging upside down on a board as a hunting trophy. Dead Eagle Owl is one of a series of comparable still lifes that...
16th-century alchemical manuscript This article is about the alchemical work. For the sound album, see Splendor Solis (album). The peacock in a crowned flask associated with Venus Splendor Solis (The Splendour of the Sun) is a version of the illuminated alchemical text attributed to Salomon Trismosin. This version dates from around 1582. The earliest version, written in Central German, is dated 1532–1535 and is part of the Kupferstichkabinett Berlin collection at the State Museums in Berlin...