Contact hypothesis

In psychology and other social sciences, the contact hypothesis suggests that intergroup contact under appropriate conditions can effectively reduce prejudice between majority and minority group members. Following WWII and the desegregation of the military and other public institutions, policymakers and social scientists had turned an eye towards the policy implications of interracial contact. Of them, social psychologist Gordon Allport united early research in this vein under intergroup contact theory.

In 1954, Allport published The Nature of Prejudice, in which he outlined the most widely cited form of the hypothesis.[1] The premise of Allport's hypothesis states that under appropriate conditions interpersonal contact could be one of the most effective ways to reduce prejudice between majority and minority group members.[1] According to Allport, properly managed contact should reduce issues of stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination that commonly occur between rival groups and lead to better intergroup interactions.

In the decades following Allport's book, social scientists expanded and applied the contact hypothesis towards the reduction of prejudice beyond racism, including prejudice towards physically and mentally disabled people, women, and LGBTQ+ people, in hundreds of different studies.[2]

In some subfields of criminology, psychology, and sociology, intergroup contact has been described as one of the best ways to improve relations among groups in conflict.[3][4] Nonetheless, the effects of intergroup contact vary widely from context to context, and empirical inquiry continues to this day.[5]

History

While Gordon W. Allport is often credited with the development of the contact hypothesis, the idea that interpersonal contact could improve intergroup relations was not a novel one. In the 1930s and 1940s, writers had already begun speculating about the outcomes of interracial contact.[6][7][8] In 1947, sociologist R. M. Williams described interpersonal collaboration with goal interdependence as a worthwhile strategy to reduce intergroup hostility.[9]

Following WWII, social scientists examined the effects of desegregation on racial attitudes in the U.S. Merchant Marine, and in desegregated New York City housing projects.[10][11] In 1951, as national attention turned to issues of desegregation in schools, ultimately leading up to Brown v. Board of Education, Robert Carter and Thurgood Marshall, from the NAACP Legal Defense and Educational Fund, solicited expert opinions from social science.[12][13] A range of social scientists, from Kenneth Clark to Floyd and Gordon Allport, weighed in on the psychological effects of desegregation, and conditions under which interracial contact might attenuate racial prejudice, including an amicus curiae brief filed in the Brown v. Board case.[14]

Other studies have claimed that contact hypothesis is very simple and optimistic and that contact would most likely gravitate toward hostility rather than friendship if two competitive parties were involved. If groups with a negative outlook were brought together, it would lead to increases of negative attitudes rather than positive.[15]

Allport situated his formulation of the contact hypothesis in broader discussion of racial diversity—a precursor to interracial proximity and contact. While diversity more generally might foment conflict and prejudice, Allport suggested that contact, under four particular conditions, would facilitate intergroup understanding and consequently reduce prejudice.

Conditions of intergroup contact

In the years prior to Allport’s framing of intergroup contact theory, social scientists had already begun discussing the conditions of intergroup contact that would produce intergroup anxiety, prejudice, or other “detrimental psychological effects”.[16] Wilner, Walkley, & Cook, two years prior to The Nature of Prejudice, studied segregation and integration in housing projects, and also suggested four conditions under which intergroup attitudes would change for the better. Under the assumption that prejudice arises from racial segregation, they suggested that it would diminish when members occupy “the same or equivalent roles in the situation,” share background characteristics like education, age, gender or socioeconomic status, perceive common interests or goals, and when the “social climate […] is not unfavorable to interracial association.”[17]

Concurrently, Carolyn Sherif and Muzafer Sherif developed their Robbers Cave experiment, an illustration of realistic conflict theory.[18] The Sherifs highlighted the importance of superordinate goals and equal status between groups, but notably, did not weigh in alongside other social scientists in their amicus brief for Brown v. Board of Education.

In Allport's own words,

"[Prejudice] may be reduced by equal status contact between majority and minority groups in the pursuit of common goals. The effect is greatly enhanced if this contact is sanctioned by institutional supports (i.e., by law, custom, or local atmosphere), and provided it is of a sort that leads to the perception of common interests and common humanity between members of the two groups."

— Gordon W. Allport, The Nature of Prejudice (1954)

In other words, four conditions under which intergroup contact will reduce prejudice are:

  • Equal status. Both groups must engage equally in the relationship. Members of the group should have similar backgrounds, qualities, and characteristics. Differences in academic backgrounds, wealth, skill, or experiences should be minimized if these qualities will influence perceptions of prestige and rank in the group.
  • Common goals. Both groups must work on a problem/task and share this as a common goal, sometimes called a superordinate goal, a goal that can only be attained if the members of two or more groups work together by pooling their efforts and resources.
  • Intergroup cooperation. Both groups must work together for their common goals without competition. Groups need to work together in the pursuit of common goals.
  • Support of authorities, law or customs. Both groups must acknowledge some authority that supports the contact and interactions between the groups. The contact should encourage friendly, helpful, egalitarian attitudes and condemn ingroup-outgroup comparisons.

Additionally, Allport specified that within intergroup cooperation, personal interaction, involving informal, personal interaction between group members would scaffold learning about each other and the formation of cross-group friendships. Yet, without these conditions, casual, or superficial, contact would cause people to resort to stereotypes.[1]

The largest meta-analysis of the contact literature suggested that the conditions are facilitating but not essential.[2] However, more recent meta-analysis highlights that many configurations of the conditions have not yet been experimentally tested.[5]

Psychological processes involved in intergroup contact

A number of psychological processes have been hypothesised to explain how and why intergroup contact is able to reduce prejudice and improve intergroup relations. Firstly, Allport (1954) argued that intergroup contact facilitates learning about the outgroup, and this new outgroup knowledge leads to prejudice reduction.[1] Secondly, intergroup contact is believed to reduce the fear and anxiety people have when interacting with the outgroup, which in turn reduces their negative evaluations of the outgroup.[19] Thirdly, intergroup contact is hypothesised to increase people's ability to take the perspective of the outgroup and empathize with their concerns.[20] Empirical research has only found weak support for role of outgroup knowledge in prejudice reduction; however, the affective mechanisms of intergroup anxiety and outgroup empathy have accumulated extensive empirical support.[21]

The reduction of prejudice through intergroup contact can be described as the reconceptualization of group categories. Allport (1954) claimed that prejudice is a direct result of generalizations and oversimplifications made about an entire group of people based on incomplete or mistaken information. The basic rationale is that prejudice may be reduced as one learns more about a category of people.[1] Rothbart and John (1985) describe belief change through contact as "an example of the general cognitive process by which attributes of category members modify category attributes" (p. 82).[22] An individual's beliefs can be modified by that person coming into contact with a culturally distinct category member and subsequently modifying or elaborating the beliefs about the category as a whole.

However, contact fails to cure conflict when contact situations create anxiety for those who take part. Contact situations need to be long enough to allow this anxiety to decrease and for the members of the conflicting groups to feel comfortable with one another.[citation needed] Additionally if the members of the two groups use this contact situation to trade insults, argue with each other, resort to physical violence, and discriminate against each other, then contact should not be expected to reduce conflict between groups. To obtain beneficial effects, the situation must include positive contact.

Effects of intergroup contact

Social scientists have documented positive effects of intergroup contact across field, experimental, and correlational studies, across a variety of contact situations, and between various social groups. Pettigrew and Tropp's canonical 2006 meta-analysis of 515 separate studies found general support for the contact hypothesis.[2] Furthermore, their analysis found that face-to-face contact between group members significantly reduced prejudice; the more contact groups had, the less prejudice group members reported.[2] Moreover, the beneficial effects of intergroup contact were significantly greater when the contact situation was structured to include Allport's facilitating conditions for optimal contact.[2]

Examples

Intergroup contact and prejudice towards African Americans

The majority of intergroup contact research has focused on reducing prejudice towards African Americans. For example, in one study, Brown, Brown, Jackson, Sellers, and Manuel (2003) investigated the amount of contact white athletes had with black teammates and whether the athletes played an individual or team sport. Team sports (e.g., football or basketball), as opposed to individual sports (e.g., track or swimming), require teamwork and cooperative interactions to win. Results showed that White athletes who played team sports reported less prejudice than athletes who played individual sports.[23]

Intergroup contact and prejudice towards homosexuality

The contact hypothesis has proven to be highly effective in alleviating prejudice directed toward homosexuals.[24] Applying the contact hypothesis to heterosexuals and homosexuals, Herek (1987) found that college students who had pleasant interactions with a homosexual tend to generalize from that experience and accept homosexuals as a group.[25] Herek and Glunt's (1993) national study of interpersonal contact and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men found that increased contact "predicted attitudes toward gay men better than did any other demographic or social psychological variable" (p. 239); such variables included gender, race, age, education, geographic residence, marital status, number of children, religion and political ideology.[26] Herek and Capitanio (1996) found that contact experiences with two or three homosexuals are associated with more favorable attitudes than are contact experiences with only one individual.[27]

Intergroup contact and prejudice towards Muslims in Europe

Savelkoul et al. (2011) in their study from the Netherlands found people living in regions with high numbers of Muslims (i.e. those more exposed to unavoidable intergroup contacts) get used to and are more experienced with their integration and express lesser perceived threats. In addition, they also found that higher contacts with Muslim colleagues directly reduce anti-Muslim attitudes.[28] Similarly, Novotny and Polonsky (2011) in their survey among Czech and Slovak university students documented that personal contacts with Muslims and experience with visiting an Islamic country associate with more positive attitudes towards Muslims.[29] However Agirdag et al. (2012) report that Belgian teachers working in schools that enroll a larger share of Muslim students have more negative attitudes toward Muslim students than other teachers.[30]

Intergroup contact with immigrant groups within the United States

Daniel J. Hopkins presented the idea that local conditions within a community or in other words changes in local immigrant demographics can affect the attitudes of people on immigrants. This can include the stereotyping of immigrants and/or a development of pro or anti-immigrant ideas. These attitudes may be shaped by experiences the non-immigrant population has with the immigrant population. He believes that this idea is not necessarily universal but that certain conditions play a role on the development of attitudes.[31]

Intergroup contact and social robots

Evidence suggests that intergroup contact theory may be applicable to social robots. Exposure to a social robot under predictable, controlled conditions can lead people to feel more positive toward that specific robot than they previously felt toward robots in general.[32] Additionally, research has shown that even imagining interacting with a robot can reduce negative feelings.[33]

Indirect intergroup contact

One of the most important advances in research on intergroup contact is the growing evidence for a number of indirect, non-face-to-face intergroup contact strategies as a means to improve relations between social groups.[34] While the benefits of direct intergroup contact have been empirically established, its implementation is often not practical. For example, in many countries, racial and religious groups are often residentially, educationally or occupationally segregated, which limits the opportunity for direct contact. However, even when the opportunity for direct intergroup contact is high, anxiety and fear can produce a negative or hostile contact experience or lead to the avoidance of the contact situation altogether.

Indirect forms of intergroup contact include:

Extended contact

The extended contact hypothesis, established by Wright and colleagues in 1997, posits that knowing that a member of one's own group has a close relationship with a member of an outgroup can lead to more positive attitudes towards that outgroup. Correlational research has demonstrated that individuals who report knowledge that an ingroup member has an outgroup friend typically report more positive outgroup attitudes, while experimental research has shown that providing ingroup members with this information creates the same positive effect.[35]

In the 20 years since its proposal, the extended contact hypothesis has guided over 100 studies, that generally find support for the positive effect of extended contact on prejudice reduction, independent of direct friendship with outgroup members.[36][37]

Parasocial contact

In a similar vein, vicarious contact involves simply observing an ingroup member interact with an outgroup member.[38] For example, positive media portrayals of intergroup interactions on television and radio, also known as the parasocial contact hypothesis, have the potential to reduce the prejudice of millions of viewers and listeners.[39]

Imagined contact

The imagined contact hypothesis was put forward by Richard J. Crisp and Rhiannon Turner (2009)[40] and proposes that simply imagining a positive encounter with a member or members of an outgroup category can promote more positive intergroup attitudes. It also proposed that imagined contact can lead to a greater desire to have social contact between groups and can help improve explicit or implicit biases toward marginalized or minority groups, such as those who are mentally or physically disabled, religious minorities, ethnic minorities, and sexual minorities.[41][42]

Electronic- or E-contact

Fiona White and her colleagues (2012; 2014) recently developed Electronic- or E-contact. E-contact involves an ingroup member interacting with an outgroup member over the Internet[43][44] and includes text-based, video-based or a mixture of both text- and video-based online interactions. Electronic contact has been empirically shown to reduce inter-religious prejudice between Christian and Muslim students in Australia in both the short[45] and long term,[46] as well between Catholic and Protestant students in Northern Ireland.[47] In the context of sexual prejudice, research also has shown that interacting online with a member of the outgroup is particularly useful as a prejudice-reduction strategy among individuals who typically report ideologically intolerant beliefs.[48] Additionally, in the context of mental health stigma, participants who experienced a brief interaction with a person diagnosed with schizophrenia reported reduced fear, anger, and stereotyping toward people with schizophrenia in general compared to a control condition.[49]

In the Latin American context, recently Rodriguez-Rivas et al. (2021) demonstrated a positive impact on the reduction of stigma towards people with mental illness in Chilean university students, following participation in a multi-component online program that incorporated electronic contact (E-contact) via videoconferencing with a person diagnosed with schizophrenia.[50]

In the Afghanistan context, recently Sahab et al. (2024) studied whether using an AI-powered software agent as a facilitator for intergroup electronic contact led to better interactions and reduced prejudice between rival ethnic groups. The findings suggest that using an AI-assisted chatbot in intergroup E-contact can enhance interaction and reduce interethnic prejudices and hostility among Afghanistan’s ethnic groups.[51]

Criticisms

While large bodies of research have been devoted to examining intergroup contact, social scientific reviews of the literature frequently voice skepticism about the likelihood of contact's optimal conditions occurring in concert, and by extension, about the generalizability of correlational research and lab studies on contact.[52]

Null findings and gaps in research

Though the general findings of intergroup contact research have inspired promise, Bertrand and Duflo (2017) find that observational correlations between intergroup contact and non-prejudiced behavior can be explained by self-selection: less prejudiced people seek out contact.[53] Comparatively, fewer controlled experimental studies of intergroup contact exist; of those that do, few measure prejudice outcomes longer than one day after treatment, leaving a gap in the literature that investigates the long-term effects of contact. Furthermore, of these experiments, none measure the reduction of racial prejudice in people over the age of 25.[5] Similarly, in a report to the United Kingdom Equality and Human Rights Commission, Dominic Abrams highlights "a dearth of good-quality longitudinal research on prejudice or prejudice reduction”.[54]

Gordon Allport himself suggested that in light of increasing racial contact in the United States, "the more contact the more trouble", unless scaffolded by the four facilitating conditions he proposed, distinguishing casual contact and "true acquaintance" or "knowledge-giving contact".[1] In political science, Allport's work is often juxtaposed with V.O. Key's examination of Southern politics, which found that racism grew in areas where the local concentrations of black Americans were higher.[55] In that context, absent the specific conditions of Allport, contact comes to produce more negative effects, namely increasing prejudice. Some social psychologists have converged with political scientists on this position.[56] Daniel J. Hopkins presented the idea that local conditions within a community (specifically changes in local immigrant demographics) can affect the attitudes of people on immigrants. This can include the stereotyping of immigrants and/or a development of pro or anti-immigrant ideas. These attitudes may be shaped by experiences the non-immigrant population has with the immigrant population. He believes that this idea is not necessarily universal but that certain conditions play a role on the development of attitudes.[57] Agirdag et al. (2012) report that Belgian teachers working in schools that enroll a larger share of Muslim students have more negative attitudes toward Muslim students than other teachers.[58] Other studies have claimed that contact hypothesis is a very simple and optimistic and that contact would most likely gravitate toward hostility rather than friendship if two competitive parties were involved. If groups with a negative outlook were brought together, it would lead to increases of negative attitudes rather than positive.[59] Furthermore, ideologies when not motivated by prejudices or negative contacts and attitudes, such as ethnic nationalism, the professed religion or the political standings, do not simply change with greater contact. For example, ecumenism or political collaboration virtually does not operate any religious dogmatic or political ideal or principle change in any of the two rival sides.

Negative contact hypothesis

Stefania Paolini, Jake Harwood, and Mark Rubin (2010) proposed that intergroup contact may have more negative than positive effects on prejudice, because it makes outgroup members' social group more salient during encounters.[60] Recent evidence suggests that although negative intergroup contact is more influential than positive intergroup contact, it is also less common than positive contact in real world intergroup encounters, in five central European countries.[61][62] Recent research also suggests that people's past experiences with out-group members moderates contact: people who have had positive experiences with out-group members in the past show a smaller discrepancy between the effects of positive and negative contact.[63]

Negative sentiments triggered by proximity are also described as NIMBYism (Not In My Back Yard).[64]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books
  2. ^ a b c d e Pettigrew, T. F.; Tropp, L. R. (2006). "A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (5): 751–783. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751. PMID 16737372. S2CID 14149856.
  3. ^ Brown, R., & Hewstone, M. (2005). An integrative theory of intergroup contact. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 37,pp. 255–343). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
  4. ^ Wright, S. C. (2009). Cross-group contact effects. In S. Otten, T. Kessler & K. Sassenberg (Eds.), Intergroup relations: The role of emotion and motivation (pp. 262–283). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  5. ^ a b c Paluck, Elizabeth Levy; Green, Seth Ariel; Green, Donald (2018). "The contact hypothesis re-evaluated". Behavioural Public Policy. 3 (2): 129–158. doi:10.1017/bpp.2018.25.
  6. ^ Baker, Paul Earnest (1934). Negro–White adjustment. New York: Association Press. p. 120.
  7. ^ Lett, H (1945). "Techniques for achieving interracial cooperation". Proceedings of the Institute on Race Relations and Community Organization. Chicago: University of Chicago and the American Council on Race Relations.
  8. ^ Brameld, T (1946). Minority problems in the public schools. New York: Harper.
  9. ^ Williams, R. M. (1947). The reduction of intergroup tensions. New York: Social Science Research Council.
  10. ^ Brophy, I. N. (1946). "The luxury of anti-Negro prejudice". Public Opinion Quarterly. 9 (4): 456–466. doi:10.1086/265762.
  11. ^ Deutsch, M; Collins, M (1951). Interracial housing: A psychological evaluation of a social experiment. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  12. ^ Kluger, Richard (2004). Simple Justice: The History of Brown v. Board of Education and Black America's Struggle for Equality. Vintage Books. ISBN 9781400030613.
  13. ^ Wrightsman, Lawrence (2008). Oral Arguments Before the Supreme Court: An Empirical Approach. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0195368628.
  14. ^ Allport, Floyd (1953). "The effects of segregation and the consequences of desegregation: A social science statement". Journal of Negro Education. 22 (1): 68–76. doi:10.2307/2293629. JSTOR 2293629.
  15. ^ Amir, Y. (1976). The role of intergroup contact in the change of prejudice and ethnic relations. In P.A. Katz (Ed.), Towards the elimination of racism (pp. 245-308).New York: Pergamon.
  16. ^ Chein, Isidor (1949). "What are the psychological effects of segregation under conditions of equal facilities?". International Journal of Opinion & Attitude Research. 3: 229–234.
  17. ^ Wilner, Daniel M.; Walkley, Rosabelle Price; Cook, Stuart W. (1952). "Residential Proximity and Intergroup Relations in Public Housing Projects". Journal of Social Issues. 8: 45–69. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1952.tb01593.x.
  18. ^ Sherif, M.; Harvey, O.J.; White, B.J.; Hood, W. & Sherif, C.W. (1961). Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment. Norman, OK: The University Book Exchange. pp. 155–184.
  19. ^ Stephan, W. G.; Stephan, C. W. (1985). "Intergroup anxiety". Journal of Social Issues. 41 (3): 157–175. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01134.x.
  20. ^ Stephan, W. G.; Finlay, K. (1999). "The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations". Journal of Social Issues. 55 (4): 729–743. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00144.
  21. ^ Pettigrew, T. F.; Tropp, L. R. (2008). "How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators". European Journal of Social Psychology. 38 (6): 922–934. doi:10.1002/ejsp.504.
  22. ^ Rothbart, M.; John, O. P. (1985). "Social categorization and behavioral episodes: A cognitive analysis of the effects of intergroup contact". Journal of Social Issues. 41 (3): 81–104. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01130.x.
  23. ^ Brown, K.T; Brown, T.N.; Jackson, J.S.; Sellers, R.M.; Manuel, W.J. (2003). "Teammates on and off the field? Contact with Black teammates and the racial attitudes of White student athletes". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 33 (7): 1379–1403. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01954.x.
  24. ^ Smith, S. J.; Axelton, A. M.; Saucier, D. A. (2009). "The effects of contact on sexual prejudice: A meta-analysis". Sex Roles. 61 (3–4): 178–191. doi:10.1007/s11199-009-9627-3. S2CID 54854303.
  25. ^ Herek, G. M. (1987). "The instrumentality of attitudes: Toward a neofunctional theory". Journal of Social Issues. 42 (2): 99–114. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1986.tb00227.x.
  26. ^ Herek, G. M.; Glunt, E. K. (1993). "Interpersonal contact and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men: Results from a national survey". Journal of Sex Research. 30 (3): 239–244. doi:10.1080/00224499309551707.
  27. ^ Herek, G. M.; Capitanio, J. P. (1996). "Some of my best friends": Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men and lesbians Personality". Social Psychology Bulletin. 22 (4): 412–424. doi:10.1177/0146167296224007. S2CID 145657548.
  28. ^ Savelkoul, Scheepers; Tolsma, J.; Hagendoorn, L. (2011). "Anti-Muslim Attitudes in The Netherlands: Tests of Contradictory Hypotheses Derived from Ethnic Competition Theory and Intergroup Contact Theory" (PDF). European Sociological Review. 27 (6): 741–758. doi:10.1093/esr/jcq035. hdl:2066/99505.
  29. ^ Novotny, J.; Polonsky, F. (2011). "The Level of Knowledge about Islam and Perception of Islam among Czech and Slovak University Students: does Ignorance Determine Subjective Attitudes?" (PDF). Sociológia. 43 (6): 674–696.
  30. ^ Agirdag, Orhan; Loobuyck, Patrick; Van Houtte, Mieke (2012). "Determinants of Attitudes Toward Muslim Students Among Flemish Teachers: A Research Note". Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 51 (2): 368–376. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2012.01637.x.
  31. ^ Hopkins, Daniel J. (2010). "Politicized Places: Explaining Where and When Immigrants Provoke Local Opposition". American Political Science Review. 104 (1): 40–60. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.500.3856. doi:10.1017/S0003055409990360. ISSN 1537-5943. S2CID 53445100.
  32. ^ Haggadone, Brad A.; Banks, Jaime; Koban, Kevin (2021-04-07). "Of robots and robotkind: Extending intergroup contact theory to social machines". Communication Research Reports. 38 (3): 161–171. doi:10.1080/08824096.2021.1909551. S2CID 233566369.
  33. ^ Wullenkord, Ricarda; Fraune, Marlena R.; Eyssel, Friederike; Šabanović, Selma (August 2016). "Getting in Touch: How imagined, actual, and physical contact affect evaluations of robots". 2016 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN). pp. 980–985. doi:10.1109/ROMAN.2016.7745228. ISBN 978-1-5090-3929-6. S2CID 6305599.
  34. ^ Dovidio, J. F.; Eller, A.; Hewstone, M. (2011). "Improving intergroup relations through direct, extended and other forms of indirect contact". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 14 (2): 147–160. doi:10.1177/1368430210390555. S2CID 146140467.
  35. ^ Wright, S. C.; Aron, A.; McLaughlin-Volpe, T.; Ropp, S. A. (1997). "The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 73: 73–90. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.73.
  36. ^ Vezzali, Loris; Hewstone, Miles; Capozza, Dora; Giovanni, Dino; Wolfer, Ralf (2014). "Improving intergroup relations with extended and vicarious forms of indirect contact". European Review of Social Psychology. 25: 314–389. doi:10.1080/10463283.2014.982948. S2CID 145419078.
  37. ^ Zhou, Shelly; Page-Gould, Elizabeth; Aron, Arthur; Moyer, Anne; Hewstone, Miles (2018). "The extended contact hypothesis: A meta-analysis on 20 years of research". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 23 (2): 132–160. doi:10.1177/1088868318762647. PMID 29671374. S2CID 4934435.
  38. ^ Mazziotta, A.; Mummendey, A.; Wright, C. S. (2011). "Vicarious intergroup contact effects: Applying social-cognitive theory to intergroup contact research" (PDF). Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 14 (2): 255–274. doi:10.1177/1368430210390533. S2CID 145338778.
  39. ^ Schiappa, E.; Gregg, P.; Hewes, D. (2005). "The Parasocial Contact Hypothesis". Communication Monographs. 72: 92–115. doi:10.1080/0363775052000342544. S2CID 16757173.
  40. ^ Crisp, R. J.; Turner, R. N. (2009). "Can imagined interactions produce positive perceptions? Reducing prejudice through simulated social contact". American Psychologist. 64 (4): 231–240. doi:10.1037/a0014718. PMID 19449982.
  41. ^ Turner, Rhiannon N.; Crisp, Richard J.; Lambert, Emily (2007). "Imagining Intergroup Contact Can Improve Intergroup Attitudes". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 10 (4): 427–441. doi:10.1177/1368430207081533. ISSN 1368-4302. S2CID 144405432.
  42. ^ Turner, Rhiannon N.; West, Keon (2012). "Behavioural consequences of imagining intergroup contact with stigmatized outgroups". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 15 (2): 193–202. doi:10.1177/1368430211418699. ISSN 1368-4302. S2CID 146535157.
  43. ^ White, F. A.; Abu-Rayya, H.; Harvey, L. J. (2015). "Improving intergroup relations in the Internet age: A critical review". Review of General Psychology. 19 (2): 129–139. doi:10.1037/gpr0000036. S2CID 149754319.
  44. ^ Amichai-Hamburger, Y.; McKenna, K. Y. (2006). "The contact hypothesis reconsidered: Interacting via the Internet". Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 11 (3): 825–843. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00037.x.
  45. ^ White, F. A.; Abu-Rayya, H. (2012). "A dual identity-electronic contact (DIEC) experiment promoting short- and long-term intergroup harmony". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 48 (3): 597–608. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.01.007.
  46. ^ White, F. A.; Abu-Rayya, H.; Weitzel, C. (2014). "Achieving twelve-months of intergroup bias reduction: The dual identity-electronic contact (DIEC) experiment". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 38: 158–163. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2013.08.002.
  47. ^ White, F. A.; Turner, R. N.; Verrelli, S.; Harvey, L. J.; Hanna, J. R. (2018). "Improving intergroup relations between Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland via E-contact" (PDF). European Journal of Social Psychology. 49 (2): 429–438. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2515. S2CID 149558151.
  48. ^ White, F. A.; Verrelli, S.; Maunder, R. D.; Kervinen, A. (2018). "Using electronic contact to reduce homonegative attitudes, emotions, and behavioral intentions among heterosexual women and men: A contemporary extension of the contact hypothesis". The Journal of Sex Research. 56 (9): 1179–1191. doi:10.1080/00224499.2018.1491943. PMID 30019950. S2CID 51678207.
  49. ^ Maunder, R. D.; White, F. A.; Verrelli, S. (2018). "Modern avenues for intergroup contact: Using E-contact and intergroup emotions to reduce stereotyping and social distancing against people with schizophrenia". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 22 (7): 947–963. doi:10.1177/1368430218794873. S2CID 150208243.
  50. ^ Rodríguez-Rivas, Matías E.; Cangas, Adolfo J.; Fuentes-Olavarría, Daniela (2021). "Controlled Study of the Impact of a Virtual Program to Reduce Stigma Among University Students Toward People With Mental Disorders". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 12: 102. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.632252. ISSN 1664-0640. PMC 7900522. PMID 33633613.
  51. ^ Sahab, Sofia; Haqbeen, Jawad; Hadfi, Rafik; Ito, Takayuki; Imade, Richard Eke; Ohnuma, Susumu; Hasegawa, Takuya (March 2024). "E-contact facilitated by conversational agents reduces interethnic prejudice and anxiety in Afghanistan". Communications Psychology. 2: 22. doi:10.1038/s44271-024-00070-z. PMC 11332027.
  52. ^ Dixon, John; Durrheim, Kevin; Tredoux, Colin (2005). "Beyond the optimal contact strategy: A reality check for the contact hypothesis". American Psychologist. 60 (7): 697–711. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.697. PMID 16221003.
  53. ^ Bertrand, Marianne; Duflo, Esther (2017). Field Experiments on Discrimination (PDF). Vol. 1. pp. 309–393. doi:10.1016/bs.hefe.2016.08.004. ISBN 9780444633248. S2CID 111405000. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  54. ^ Abrams, Dominic (2010). "Processes of prejudice: Theory, evidence and intervention". Human Rights: 68.
  55. ^ Key, V. O. (1949). Southern Politics in State and Nation. University of Tennessee Press. ISBN 9780870494352.
  56. ^ Enos, Ryan (2017). The Space between Us: Social Geography and Politics. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1108420648.
  57. ^ Hopkins, Daniel J. (2010). "Politicized Places: Explaining Where and When Immigrants Provoke Local Opposition". American Political Science Review. 104 (1): 40–60. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.500.3856. doi:10.1017/S0003055409990360. ISSN 1537-5943. S2CID 53445100.
  58. ^ Agirdag, Orhan; Loobuyck, Patrick; Van Houtte, Mieke (2012). "Determinants of Attitudes Toward Muslim Students Among Flemish Teachers: A Research Note". Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 51 (2): 368–376. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2012.01637.x.
  59. ^ Amir, Y. (1976). The role of intergroup contact in the change of prejudice and ethnic relations. In P.A. Katz (Ed.), Towards the elimination of racism (pp. 245-308).New York: Pergamon.
  60. ^ Paolini, S.; Harwood, J.; Rubin, M. (2010). "Negative intergroup contact makes group memberships salient: Explaining why intergroup conflict endures". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (12): 1723–1738. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1004.4213. doi:10.1177/0146167210388667. PMID 21051766. S2CID 23829651.
  61. ^ Barlow, F. K.; Paolini, S.; Pedersen, A.; Hornsey, M. J.; Radke, H. R. M.; Harwood, J.; Rubin, M.; Sibley, C. G. (2012). "The contact caveat: Negative contact predicts increased prejudice more than positive contact predicts reduced prejudice". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 38 (12): 1629–1643. doi:10.1177/0146167212457953. hdl:1959.13/941028. PMID 22941796. S2CID 24346499.
  62. ^ Graf, S.; Paolini, S.; Rubin, M. (2014). "Negative intergroup contact is more influential, but positive intergroup contact is more common: Assessing contact prominence and contact prevalence in five Central European countries". European Journal of Social Psychology. 44 (6): 536–547. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2052. hdl:1959.13/1054245.
  63. ^ Paolini, S.; Harwood, J.; Rubin, M.; Husnu, S.; Joyce, N.; Hewstone, M. (2014). "Positive and extensive intergroup contact in the past buffers against the disproportionate impact of negative contact in the present". European Journal of Social Psychology. 44 (6): 548–562. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2029. hdl:1959.13/1054400.
  64. ^ Solodoch, Omer (2024). "Overburdened? How refugee dispersal policies can mitigate NIMBYism and public backlash". Journal of European Public Policy. 31 (9): 2455–2482. doi:10.1080/13501763.2023.2191271. ISSN 1350-1763.

Read other articles:

Kedaung A bed of blue mussels, Mytilus edulis, in the intertidal zone in Cornwall, England SubclassesPteriomorphia (marine mussels) Palaeoheterodonta (freshwater mussels) Heterodonta (zebra mussels)lbs Kedaung ( /ˈmʌsəl/) adalah nama umum yang digunakan untuk anggota beberapa famili moluska kerang, dari habitat air asin dan air tawar . Kelompok-kelompok ini memiliki kesamaan cangkang yang bentuknya memanjang dan asimetris dibandingkan dengan kerang lain yang dapat dimakan, yang seringkali ...

 

 

Artikel ini perlu diwikifikasi agar memenuhi standar kualitas Wikipedia. Anda dapat memberikan bantuan berupa penambahan pranala dalam, atau dengan merapikan tata letak dari artikel ini. Untuk keterangan lebih lanjut, klik [tampil] di bagian kanan. Mengganti markah HTML dengan markah wiki bila dimungkinkan. Tambahkan pranala wiki. Bila dirasa perlu, buatlah pautan ke artikel wiki lainnya dengan cara menambahkan [[ dan ]] pada kata yang bersangkutan (lihat WP:LINK untuk keterangan lebih lanjut...

 

 

Synagogue in Lebanon Deir el Qamar Synagogueكنيس دير القمرReligionAffiliationOrthodox JudaismRiteSephardicEcclesiastical or organizational statusopenLocationLocationChouf, LebanonArchitectureCompleted1638 The Deir el Qamar Synagogue, in Deir el Qamar, a village in south-central Lebanon, is the oldest synagogue in Mount Lebanon.[1] The synagogue was built in the 17th century, during the Ottoman era in Lebanon, to serve the local Jewish population, some of whom were part of ...

يفتقر محتوى هذه المقالة إلى الاستشهاد بمصادر. فضلاً، ساهم في تطوير هذه المقالة من خلال إضافة مصادر موثوق بها. أي معلومات غير موثقة يمكن التشكيك بها وإزالتها. (ديسمبر 2018)   ميّز عن جامع سنان باشا (إسطنبول).   ميّز عن جامع سنان باشا (دمشق).   هذه المقالة عن جامع سنا�...

 

 

BungkalKecamatanPeta lokasi Kecamatan BungkalNegara IndonesiaProvinsiJawa TimurKabupatenPonorogoPemerintahan • CamatDrs.Sumarno.Populasi • Total68,095 jiwaKode Kemendagri35.02.03 Kode BPS3502030 Luas- 32876 ha;Desa/kelurahan20 Bungkal adalah sebuah Kecamatan di Kabupaten Ponorogo, Provinsi Jawa Timur, Indonesia. Letaknya berada di sebelah selatan sekitar 20 km dari pusat Kota Ponorogo. Batas wilayah Utara Kecamatan Jetis dan Kecamatan Balong Timur Kecamatan S...

 

 

FilefjellFillefjellHighest pointPeakHøgeloftElevation1,920 m (6,300 ft)GeographyFilefjell CountryNorwayDistrictsVestland, Innlandet and BuskerudRiversSmeddalselvi and BegnaRange coordinates61°09′N 8°15′E / 61.150°N 8.250°E / 61.150; 8.250 Filefjell is a mountainous area in Norway. It is located between Lærdal in the Vestland county and Valdres in Innlandet county. It is the historical, as well as modern, main route, linking Western Norway and E...

Resistance of a fluid to shear deformation ViscosityA simulation of liquids with different viscosities. The liquid on the left has lower viscosity than the liquid on the right.Common symbolsη, μDerivations fromother quantitiesμ = G·tDimension M L − 1 T − 1 {\displaystyle {\mathsf {M}}{\mathsf {L}}^{-1}{\mathsf {T}}^{-1}} Part of a series onContinuum mechanics J = − D d φ d x {\displaystyle J=-D{\frac {d\varphi }{dx}}} Fick's laws of diffusion Laws Conservations...

 

 

Fubine Monferratocomune Fubine Monferrato – VedutaVeduta LocalizzazioneStato Italia Regione Piemonte Provincia Alessandria AmministrazioneSindacoLino Pettazzi (Lega Nord; lista civica Fubine Porta del Monferrato) dall'11-6-2018 (2º mandato dal 15-5-2023) TerritorioCoordinate44°57′49″N 8°25′52″E / 44.963611°N 8.431111°E44.963611; 8.431111 (Fubine Monferrato)Coordinate: 44°57′49″N 8°25′52″E / 44.963611°N 8...

 

 

この項目には、一部のコンピュータや閲覧ソフトで表示できない文字が含まれています(詳細)。 数字の大字(だいじ)は、漢数字の一種。通常用いる単純な字形の漢数字(小字)の代わりに同じ音の別の漢字を用いるものである。 概要 壱万円日本銀行券(「壱」が大字) 弐千円日本銀行券(「弐」が大字) 漢数字には「一」「二」「三」と続く小字と、「壱」「�...

Adresse bondyblog.fr Type de site actualité Langue français Propriétaire Association Bondy Blog Rédacteur en chef Héléna Berkaoui Directeur de la publication Sarah Ichou État actuel actif modifier  Le Bondy Blog est un média en ligne créé le 11 novembre 2005. Il a pour vocation de donner la parole aux habitants des banlieues urbaines. Histoire Ancien logo. Le Bondy Blog a été ouvert par Serge Michel, du magazine suisse L'Hebdo, pendant les émeutes de 2005 dans les banlieues...

 

 

Airline in French Guiana GUY redirects here. For the Lady Gaga song, see G.U.Y. Air Guyane Express IATA ICAO Callsign 3S GUY GREEN BIRD Founded2002HubsCayenne-Rochambeau AirportFleet size3Destinations8HeadquartersCayenne-Rochambeau Airport Matoury, French Guiana, FranceWebsitehttps://www.airguyane.com/ Air Guyane SP is a French-Guyanese airline with its head office on the grounds of Cayenne-Rochambeau Airport in Matoury, French Guiana, France.[1] It operates regional scheduled flights...

 

 

UK-based supplier of gas and electricity Innogy plc redirects here. For the renewable energy subsidiary of E.ON, see Innogy. Npower LimitedFormerlyNational Power PLCInnogy PLCRWE NpowerNpowerNpower Business SolutionsCompany typeLimited companyIndustryEnergy to BusinessesFounded2000 (as Innogy plc) 2002 (as RWE Npower) 2016 (as Npower Limited)Defunct2021FateMerged into E.ON NextSuccessorE.ON NextHeadquartersSwindon, England, United KingdomProductsGas and electricityNumber of employees5,700Pare...

34°13′26″N 4°00′24″W / 34.22389°N 4.00667°W / 34.22389; -4.00667 إقليم تازة علم [[ملف:|200px]] العاصمة تازة البلد  المغرب الجهة {{{جهة}}} عدد السكان 528.419 [1] فردا الإحصاء 2014 المساحة 6.097 كلم² الكثافة 91,6 فرد/كلم² الجهة الإدارية جهة فاس مكناس MA03 المقاطعة {{{مقاطعة}}} المطار المدني الأقرب {{{م...

 

 

Canadian province party leader election 2023 Ontario New Democratic Party leadership election ← 2009 February 4, 2023   Candidate Marit Stiles Riding Davenport First ballot Acclaimed Leader before election Peter Tabuns (Interim) Elected Leader Marit Stiles 2023 Ontario New Democratic Party leadership electionDateFebruary 4, 2023ConventionMetro Toronto Convention Centre, Toronto, Ontario[1]Resigning leaderAndrea HorwathWon byMarit StilesBallots0 (acclamation)Candid...

 

 

2001 compilation album by Tom WaitsUsed Songs 1973–1980Compilation album by Tom WaitsReleasedOctober 23, 2001.LabelRhino RecordsProducerBones Howe Used Songs 1973–1980 is a compilation of songs from Tom Waits's Asylum Records years. Track listing Songs written by Tom Waits Heartattack and Vine - 4:44 from Heartattack and Vine (1980) Tom Waits - vocals, electric guitar Larry Taylor - bass John Thomassie - drums Plas Johnson - tenor and baritone saxes Eggs and Sausage (In a Cadillac...

Radio station in Milwaukee, WisconsinWLUM-FMMilwaukee, WisconsinBroadcast areaGreater MilwaukeeFrequency102.1 MHzBrandingFM 102/1ProgrammingFormatAlternative rockOwnershipOwnerMilwaukee Radio Alliance(Milwaukee Radio Alliance, LLC)Sister stationsWLDB, WZTIHistoryFirst air dateSeptember 1960; 63 years ago (1960-09)Former call signsWMKE (1960–1964)WAWA-FM (1964–1979)Call sign meaningWe Love YoU, MilwaukeeTechnical information[1]Licensing authorityFCCFacility ...

 

 

American Republican politician. Bob KulpMember of the Wisconsin State Assemblyfrom the 69th districtIn officeDecember 4, 2013 – January 4, 2021Preceded byScott SuderSucceeded byDonna Rozar Personal detailsBorn (1966-03-21) March 21, 1966 (age 58)Elkhart, IndianaPolitical partyRepublicanSpouseLauraChildren7ResidenceStratford, WisconsinAlma materThomas Edison State UniversityProfessionPoliticianWebsiteOfficial twitter Bob Kulp (March 21, 1966) is an American businessman and poli...

 

 

Telephone numbers in Germany8 geographic zonesLocationCountryGermanyContinentEuropeRegulatorFederal Network AgencyTypeOpenNSN length3 to 13[1]Format(xx…) xx…Access codesCountry code49International access00Long-distance0List of Germany dialing codes The regulation of telephone numbers in Germany is the responsibility of the Federal Network Agency (German: Bundesnetzagentur, BNetzA) of the German government. The agency has a mandate to telecommunications in Germany and other infras...

Japanese figure skater and coach Nobuo SatoSato in 2010Born (1942-01-03) January 3, 1942 (age 82)Osaka, JapanFigure skating careerCountry JapanJapanese nameKanji佐藤信夫Kanaさとう のぶおTranscriptionsRomanizationSatō Nobuo Nobuo Satō (佐藤 信夫, Satō Nobuo, born January 3, 1942 in Osaka) is a Japanese figure skater and coach. He is a ten-time Japanese national champion. He represented Japan at the 1960 Winter Olympics, where he placed 14th, and at the 1964 Winter Ol...

 

 

  提示:此条目页的主题不是中華民國—新加坡關係。   關於其他「中」字國家與「新」字國家的外交關係,詳見中新關係 (消歧義)。 中国—新加坡關係 中华人民共和国 新加坡 代表機構中國駐新加坡大使館新加坡驻华大使馆代表大使 曹忠明大使 陈海泉[1] 中國—新加坡關係(英語:China–Singapore relations)即歷史上的中國和新加坡、以至現在中華人民共...