Study of if and how individuals yielded to or defied a majority group
In psychology, the Asch conformity experiments or the Asch paradigm were a series of studies directed by Solomon Asch studying if and how individuals yielded to or defied a majority group and the effect of such influences on beliefs and opinions.[1][2][3][4]
Many early studies in social psychology were adaptations of earlier work on "suggestibility" whereby researchers such as Edward L. Thorndyke were able to shift the preferences of adult subjects towards majority or expert opinion.[3] Still the question remained as to whether subject opinions were actually able to be changed, or if such experiments were simply documenting a Hawthorne effect in which participants simply gave researchers the answers they wanted to hear. Solomon Asch's experiments on group conformity mark a departure from these earlier studies by removing investigator influence from experimental conditions.
In 1951, Asch conducted his first conformity laboratory experiments at Swarthmore College, laying the foundation for his remaining conformity studies. The experiment was published on two occasions.[1][11]
Method
Groups of eight male college students participated in a simple "perceptual" task. In reality, all but one of the participants were actors, and the true focus of the study was about how the remaining participant would react to the actors' behavior.
The actors knew the true aim of the experiment, but were introduced to the subject as other participants. Each student viewed a card with a line on it, followed by another with three lines labeled A, B, and C (see accompanying figure). One of these lines was identical in length to that on the first card, and the other two lines were clearly longer or shorter (i.e., a near-100% rate of correct responding was expected). Each participant was then asked to say aloud which line matched the length of that on the first card. Before the experiment, all actors were given detailed instructions on how they should respond to each trial (card presentation). They would always unanimously nominate one comparator, but on certain trials they would give the correct response and on others, an incorrect response. The group was seated such that the real participant always responded last.
Subjects completed 18 trials. On the first two trials, both the subject and the actors gave the obvious, correct answer. On the third trial, the actors would all give the same wrong answer. This wrong-responding recurred on 11 of the remaining 15 trials. It was subjects' behavior on these 12 "critical trials" (the 3rd trial + the 11 trials where the actors gave the same wrong answer) that formed the aim of the study: to test how many subjects would change their answer to conform to those of the 7 actors, despite it being wrong. Subjects were interviewed after the study including being debriefed about the true purpose of the study. These post-test interviews shed valuable light on the study—both because they revealed subjects often were "just going along", and because they revealed considerable individual differences to Asch. Additional trials with slightly altered conditions were also run,[1] including having a single actor also give the correct answer.
Asch's experiment also had a condition in which participants were tested alone with only the experimenter in the room. In total, there were 50 subjects in the experimental condition and 37 in the control condition.
Results
In the control group, with no pressure to conform to actors, the error rate on the critical stimuli was less than 0.7%.[1]
In the actor condition also, the majority of participants' responses remained correct (64.3%), but a sizable minority of responses conformed to the actors' (incorrect) answer (35.7%). The responses revealed strong individual differences: 12% of participants followed the group in nearly all of the tests. 26% of the sample consistently defied majority opinion, with the rest conforming on some trials. An examination of all critical trials in the experimental group revealed that one-third of all responses were incorrect. These incorrect responses often matched the incorrect response of the majority group (i.e., actors). Overall, 74% of participants gave at least one incorrect answer out of the 12 critical trials.[1] Regarding the study results, Asch stated: "That intelligent, well-meaning young people are willing to call white black is a matter of concern."
Interview responses
Participants' interview responses revealed a complex mixture of individual differences in subjects' reaction to the experimental situation, with distinct reactions linked to factors such as confidence, self-doubt, the desire to be normative, and resolving perceived confusion over the nature of the task.
Asch's report included interviews of a subject that remained "independent" and another that "yielded". Each provided a descriptive account following disclosure of the true nature of the experiment. The "independent" subject said that he felt happy and relieved and added, "I do not deny that at times I had the feeling: 'to go with it[...] I'll go along with the rest.'"[12]: 182 At the other end of the spectrum, one "yielding" subject (who conformed in 11 of 12 critical trials) said, "I suspected about the middle—but tried to push it out of my mind."[12]: 182 Asch points out that although the "yielding" subject was suspicious, he was not sufficiently confident to go against the majority.
Attitudes of independent responders
Subjects who did not conform to the majority reacted either with "confidence": they experienced conflict between their idea of the obvious answer and the group's incorrect answer, but stuck with their own answer, or were "withdrawn". These latter subjects stuck with their perception but did not experience conflict in doing so. Some participants also exhibited "doubt", responding in accordance with their perception, but questioning their own judgment while nonetheless sticking to their (correct) response, expressing this as needing to behave as they had been asked to do in the task.
Attitudes of responders conforming on one or more trials
Participants who conformed to the majority on at least 50% of trials reported reacting with what Asch called a "distortion of perception". These participants, who made up a distinct minority (only 12 subjects), expressed the belief that the actors' answers were correct, and were apparently unaware that the majority were giving incorrect answers.
Among the other participants who yielded on some trials, most expressed what Asch termed "distortion of judgment". These participants concluded after a number of trials that they must be wrongly interpreting the stimuli and that the majority must be right, leading them to answer with the majority. These individuals were characterized by low levels of confidence. The final group of participants who yielded on at least some trials exhibited a "distortion of action". These subjects reported that they knew what the correct answer was, but conformed with the majority group simply because they didn't want to seem out of step by not going along with the rest.[12] All conforming respondents underestimated the frequency with which they conformed to the majority.[3]
Variations on the original paradigm
In subsequent research experiments, Asch explored several variations on the paradigm from his 1951 study.[2]
In 1955 he reported on work with 123 male students from three different universities.[3] A second paper in 1956 also consisted of 123 male college students from three different universities.[4] Asch did not state if this was in fact the same sample as reported in his 1955 paper; the principal difference is that the 1956 paper includes an elaborate account of his interviews with participants. Across all these papers, Asch found the same results: participants conformed to the majority group in about one-third of all critical trials.
Presence of a true partner
Asch found that the presence of a "true partner" (a "real" participant or another actor told to give the correct response to each question) decreased conformity.[1][3] In studies that had one actor give correct responses to the questions, only 5% of the participants continued to answer with the majority.[13] In subsequent interviews, subjects claimed a degree of "warmth" and "closeness" towards the partner, and attributed an increase in confidence to their presence. Still, subjects rejected the notion that it was the partner who allowed them to answer independently.
Partner dissent and accuracy
Experiments were also designed to determine if the partner effect on subject conformity was due to the partner's dissent from the majority or their accuracy in answering questions.[3][4] In one experiment, Asch identified two classes of dissenter: "extremist" (under this condition, dissenters always chose the worst of the comparison lines and the majority chose the line closest to the standard in length) and "compromising" (dissenter: closest to standard; majority: worst comparison line). In compromising dissenter trials, subject conformity decreased overall and when they did conform, they conformed to the dissenter, not the majority. Compromising dissenters were seen to control the "choice of errors". In trials with an extremist dissenter, subject conformity decreased dramatically with only 9% of respondents continuing to answer with the majority. Therefore, partner dissent was found to increase independence, moderating errors (conformity).
Withdrawal of a partner
Asch also examined whether the removal of a true partner partway through the experiment influenced participants' level of conformity.[1][3] He found low levels of conformity during the first half of the experiment. However, halfway through the experiment the partner rejoined the majority, answering in lockstep with the group. When their partner switched, the subject conformity rose to levels consistent to if they had never had a partner at all. Asch classified this finding as a "desertion" effect. In a variant of this study, the partner left the experiment halfway-through altogether (an excuse was provided for their departure). Under these conditions, the partner's influence lingered through the second half of the experiment; the subject's conformity to the group increased after the partner's departure, but not as drastically if the partner was perceived as having switched sides.[3]
Majority size
Asch also examined whether decreasing or increasing the majority size had an influence on participants' level of conformity.[1][2][3] When paired with a single individual who opposed their answers, the subject retained high levels of independence in their answers. Increasing the opposing group to two or three persons increased conformity substantially. Increases beyond three persons (e.g., four, five, six, etc.) did not further-increase conformity.
Written responses
Asch also varied the method of participants' responding in studies where actors verbalized their responses aloud but the "real" participant responded in writing at the end of each trial. Conformity significantly decreased when shifting from public to written responses.[4]
Degree of wrongness
Another research question examined by Asch was whether varying the magnitude of majority "wrongness" affected subject conformity to group norms.[3] To answer this question, the difference between the reference line and three comparison lines was systematically increased to determine if there was a point where the extremity of the majority's error affected subject conformity. The authors failed to find a point at which subject conformity to the majority was completely eliminated, even when the disparity between lines was increased to 7 inches.
----The Role of the Asch Experiment and Social Interaction In the replication of the Asch Experiment, particularly in studies within the field of economics, the impact of monetary incentives has been investigated. Although the rewards used in the replication may bear less resemblance to real-life decision-making processes, important findings regarding the influence of monetary incentives on group pressure have been obtained.[14]The Influence of Political Views Group pressure's impact on political views has been explored in settings akin to the Asch Experiment. These inquiries have demonstrated that group pressure can sway political opinions. The Crutchfield Experiment is noteworthy in this regard as it serves as a tool to comprehend the social sway on political stances. The Crutchfield Experiment elucidates how individuals respond under group pressure and the extent to which their perceptions can be swayed.[15] Participants find themselves alone in a scenario where they evaluate a series of light combinations; however, they are actually privy to the responses of other participants and tend to adhere to group norms. This experiment furnishes a pivotal model for grasping the social influence on political views.[16]The Role of Individual Differences
Lastly, studies have examined personality traits that resist or succumb to group pressure in Asch experiments. Individual characteristics such as intelligence, self-esteem, and the need for social approval have been scrutinized for their impact on conformity.[17] These studies have illustrated that different personality traits exert varying effects on conformity under group pressure.Individual differences are a topic that allows the study of unique characteristics and behavioral differences between individuals. Researchers such as Crutchfield and others have examined the relationship between personality traits and compliance behavior. For example, low agreeableness rates have been found to be associated with characteristics such as intellectual competence and leadership ability. However, the relationship between intelligence and adaptation is not clear. The effects of self-esteem and the need for social approval on adjustment have also been investigated.[16] The effect of gender on adjustment has also been examined, but the results are conflicting. "Research in this area makes an important contribution to understanding differences between individuals.
Interpretations
Normative influence vs. referent informational influence
The Asch conformity experiments are often interpreted as evidence for the power of conformity and normative social influence,[18][19][20] where normative influence is the willingness to conform publicly to attain social reward and avoid social punishment.[21] From this perspective, the results are viewed as a striking example of people publicly endorsing the group response despite knowing full well that they were endorsing an incorrect response.[22][23]
Similarly, Jerry M. Burger admits the normative influence effect of the experiment in Chapter 21 of Noba online book.[24] He mentioned that people follow the crowd to avoid potential criticism. During Asch's experiment, participants choose the wrong answer to keep the association with the group. The demonstration in this experiment broadens people's understanding of the large application of normative influence. To stay consistent with other group members, people may follow a trend that is apparently wrong. Moreover, the behavior of normative conformity may reduce when the individual response is not accessible to other people.[25] This phenomenon further stresses the social role in normative influence.
In contrast, John Turner and colleagues argue that the interpretation of the Asch conformity experiments as normative influence is inconsistent with the data.[18][19][20] They point out that post-experiment interviews revealed that participants experienced uncertainty about their judgement during the experiments. Although the correct answer appeared obvious to the researchers, this was not necessarily the experience of participants. Subsequent research has demonstrated similar patterns of conformity where participants were anonymous and thus not subject to social punishment or reward on the basis of their responses.[26] From this perspective, the Asch conformity experiments are viewed as evidence for the self-categorization theory account of social influence (otherwise known as the theory of referent informational influence).[18][19][20][27][28][29] Here, the observed conformity is an example of depersonalization processes, whereby people expect to hold the same opinions as others in their ingroup and will often adopt those opinions.
The conformity demonstrated in Asch experiments may contradict aspects of social comparison theory.[18][19][30] Social comparison theory suggests that, when seeking to validate opinions and abilities, people will first turn to direct observation. If direct observation is ineffective or not available, people will then turn to comparable others for validation.[31] In other words, social comparison theory predicts that social reality testing will arise when physical reality testing yields uncertainty. The Asch conformity experiments demonstrate that uncertainty can arise as an outcome of social reality testing. More broadly, this inconsistency has been used to support the position that the theoretical distinction between social reality testing and physical reality testing is untenable.[19][20][32][33]
Selective representation in textbooks and the media
Asch's 1956 report emphasized the predominance of independence over yielding saying "the facts that were being judged were, under the circumstances, the most decisive."[4] However, a 1990 survey of US social psychology textbooks found that most ignored independence, instead reported a misleading summary of the results as reflecting complete power of the situation to produce conformity of behavior and belief.[34]
A 2015 survey found no change, with just 1 of 20 major texts reporting that most participant-responses defied majority opinion. No text mentioned that 95% of subjects defied the majority at least once. Nineteen of the 20 books made no mention of Asch's interview data in which many participants said they were certain all along that the actors were wrong.[35] This portrayal of the Asch studies was suggested to fit with social psychology narratives of situationism, obedience and conformity, to the neglect of recognition of disobedience of immoral commands (e.g., disobedience shown by participants in Milgram Studies), desire for fair treatment (e.g., resistance to tyranny shown by many participants in the Stanford prison studies) and self-determination.[35]
^ abcdeAsch, S.E. (1956). "Studies of independence and conformity. A minority of one against a unanimous majority". Psychological Monographs. 70 (9): 1–70. doi:10.1037/h0093718.
^Pasupathi, M (1999). "Age differed in response to conformity pressure for emotional and nonemotional material". Psychology and Aging. 14 (1): 170–74. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.14.1.170. PMID10224640.
^Cooper, H.M. (1979). "Statistically combined independent studies: A meta-analysis of sex differences in conformity research". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 37: 131–146. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.131.
^Eagly, A.H.; Carli, L. (1981). "Sex of researchers and sex-typed communications as determinants of sex differed in influenceability: A meta-analysis of social influence studies". Psychological Bulletin. 90 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.1.1.
^Asch, S. E. (1952a). Effects of group pressure on the modification and distortion of judgments. In G. E. Swanson, T. M. Newcomb & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (2nd ed., pp. 2–11). New York: Holt.
^ abcAsch, S.E. (1951). Effects of group pressure on the modification and distortion of judgments. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership and men (pp. 177–190). Pittsburgh, PA:Carnegie Press.
^Morris; Miller (1975). "The effects of consensus-breaking and consensus-preempting partners on reduction in conformity". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 11 (3): 215–223. doi:10.1016/s0022-1031(75)80023-0.
^ abcdTurner, J.C. (1985). Lawler, E. J (ed.). "Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior". Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research. 2. Greenwich, CT: 77–122.
^ abcdeTurner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D. & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell
^ abcdTurner, J. C. (1991). Social influence. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.
^Deutsch, M.; Harold, G. (1955). "A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgement". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 51 (3): 629–636. doi:10.1037/h0046408. PMID13286010.
^Aronson, T. D.; Wilson, R. M.; Akert, E. (2010). Social Psychology (7 ed.). Pearson.
^Bond, R., & Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch’s (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 111–137.
^Hogg, M. A.; Turner, J. C. (1987). Doise, W.; Moscivici, S. (eds.). "Social identity and conformity: A theory of referent informational influence". Current Issues in European Social Psychology. 2. Cambridge: 139–182.
^Turner, J.C. (1982). Tajfel, H. (ed.). "Toward a cognitive redefinition of the social group". Social Identity and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge, UK: 15–40.
^Haslam, A. S. (2001). Psychology in Organizations. London, SAGE Publications.
^Haslam, S. Alexander; Reicher, Stephen D.; Platow, Michael J. (2011). The new psychology of leadership: Identity, influence and power. New York, NY: Psychology Press. ISBN978-1-84169-610-2.
^Turner, J. C.; Oakes, P. J. (1997). McGarty, C.; Haslam, S. A. (eds.). "The socially structured mind". The Message of Social Psychology. Cambridge, MA: 355–373.
^Friend, R.; Rafferty, Y.; Bramel, D. (1990). "A puzzling misinterpretation of the Asch 'conformity' study". European Journal of Social Psychology. 20: 29–44. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420200104.
^ abGriggs, R. A. (2015). "The Disappearance of Independence in Textbook Coverage of Asch's Social Pressure Experiments". Teaching of Psychology. 42 (2): 137–142. doi:10.1177/0098628315569939. S2CID146908363.
Asch, S. E. (1940). "Studies in the principles of judgments and attitudes: II. Determination of judgments by group and by ego-standards". Journal of Social Psychology. 12 (2): 433–465. doi:10.1080/00224545.1940.9921487.
Asch, S. E. (1948). "The doctrine of suggestion, prestige and imitation in social psychology". Psychological Review. 55 (5): 250–276. doi:10.1037/h0057270. PMID18887584.
Asch S. E. (1946). "Forming impressions of personality". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258–290.
Coffin, E. E. (1941). "Some conditions of suggestion and suggestibility: A study of certain attitudinal and situational factors influencing the process of suggestion". Psychological Monographs. 53 (4): i-125. doi:10.1037/h0093490.
Lewis, H.B. (1941). "Studies in the principles of judgements and attitudes: IV. The operation of prestige suggestion". Journal of Social Psychology. 14: 229–256. doi:10.1080/00224545.1941.9921508.
Lorge, I (1936). "Prestige, suggestion, and attitudes". Journal of Social Psychology. 7 (4): 386–402. doi:10.1080/00224545.1936.9919891.
Miller, N.E. & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Moore, H.T. (1921). "The comparative influence of majority and expert opinion". American Journal of Psychology. 32 (1): 16–20. doi:10.2307/1413472. JSTOR1413472.
Sherif, M.A. (1935). "A study of some social factors in perception". Archives of Psychology. 27: 1–60.
Thorndike, E. L. The psychology of wants, interests, and attitudes. New York, NY: D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc.
RyavallenRyavallen pada tahun 2008LokasiBorås, SwediaTipeLapangan olahragaKapasitas19.400Dibuka1941 (1941)PemakaiIF Elfsborg (dahulu), Norrby IF Ryavallen adalah sebuah stadion yang terletak di Borås, Swedia. Stadion ini umumnya dipergunakan untuk menggelar pertandingan sepak bola dan menjadi markas Norrby IF. Stadion ini memiliki daya tampung 19.400 orang. Stadion ini menjadi salah satu lokasi penyelenggaraan Piala Dunia FIFA 1958. Galeri Referensi Wikimedia Commons memiliki media men...
33rd Chief Justice of India Vishweshwar Nath KhareChancellor of the Central University of JharkhandIncumbentAssumed office August 2017Appointed byRam Nath KovindJudge of Supreme Court of IndiaIn office21 March 1997 – 01 May 2004Appointed byShankar Dayal Sharma33rd Chief Justice of IndiaAppointed byA. P. J. KalamPreceded byGopal Ballav PattanaikSucceeded byS. Rajendra Babu Personal detailsBorn (1939-05-02) 2 May 1939 (age 84)Allahabad, Uttar Pradesh, IndiaAlma materAlla...
U.S. federal law enforcement agency FBI redirects here. For other uses, see FBI (disambiguation). Federal Bureau of InvestigationFederal Bureau of Investigation's sealFBI special agent badgeFlag of the Federal Bureau of InvestigationAbbreviationFBIMottoFidelity, Bravery, IntegrityAgency overviewFormedJuly 26, 1908 (as the Bureau of Investigation)Employees≈35,000[1]Annual budgetUS$9,748,829,000 (FY 2021)[2]Jurisdictional structureFederal agencyUnited StatesOperations jurisdic...
عطاف - قرية مصرية - قرى مركز المحلة الكبرى تقسيم إداري البلد مصر المحافظة محافظة الغربية المركز المحلة الكبرى وحدة محلية محلة حسن المسؤولون رئيس الوحدة سناء حسن الشهاوي[1] السكان التعداد السكاني 5,192 نسمة (إحصاء 2006) معلومات أخرى التوقيت ت ع م+02:00 الرمز ا�...
新加坡政府機構National Arts Council機構概要成立時間1991年10月15日,32年前(1991-10-15)前身機構Singapore Cultural FoundationCultural Division of Ministry of Community DevelopmentFestival of Arts SecretariatNational Theatre Trust機構類型新加坡政府法定机构机构驻地90 Goodman Road, Goodman Arts Centre, Blk A #01-01, Singapore 439053隸屬部長唐振輝上级机构新加坡文化、社區及青年部網站www.nac.gov.sg 國家藝術理�...
Entrepreneur and politician from Chicago, Illinois This biography of a living person needs additional citations for verification. Please help by adding reliable sources. Contentious material about living persons that is unsourced or poorly sourced must be removed immediately from the article and its talk page, especially if potentially libelous.Find sources: Tom Tunney – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (November 2008) (Learn how and when to...
Феодосий Антонович Иотка Дата рождения 9 мая 1913(1913-05-09) Место рождения Мельниково, Российская империя, ныне Новичихинский район Дата смерти 31 октября 1990(1990-10-31) (77 лет) Место смерти Киев Принадлежность СССР Сражения/войны Великая Отечественная война Награды и прем�...
This article includes a list of references, related reading, or external links, but its sources remain unclear because it lacks inline citations. Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations. (April 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) 1998 filmBirdcage InnPoster for Birdcage InnHangul파란 대문Revised RomanizationParan daemunMcCune–ReischauerP‘aran taemun Directed byKim Ki-dukWritten byKim Ki-dukSeo Jeong-minProduced byLee Seung-jaeSt...
2013 studio album by The Idea of NorthSmileStudio album by The Idea of NorthReleased12 August 2013StudioJames Morrison StudiosLength55:47LabelABC JazzProducerNick Begbie, The Idea of NorthThe Idea of North chronology This Christmas(2012) Smile(2013) Anthology(2014) Smile is the ninth studio album by Australian vocal ensemble, The Idea of North. The album was released on 12 August 2013[1] and peaked at number 86 on the ARIA Charts. At the ARIA Music Awards of 2013, the album wo...
Place in Kuala Lumpur, MalaysiaBukit Jalil بوکيت جليل武吉加里爾புக்கிட் ஜலீல்Lebuhraya Bukit Jalil in Bukit Jalil.Bukit JalilLocation within MalaysiaCoordinates: 3°3′31″N 101°41′31″E / 3.05861°N 101.69194°E / 3.05861; 101.69194CountryMalaysiaFederal TerritoryKuala LumpurConstituencySeputehGovernment • Local AuthorityDewan Bandaraya Kuala Lumpur • MayorMhd Amin Nordin Abdul AzizTime zoneUTC+8 (M...
Small nucleolar RNA snR61/Z1/Z11Predicted secondary structure and sequence conservation of snosnR61IdentifiersSymbolsnosnR61RfamRF00476Other dataRNA typeGene; snRNA; snoRNA; CD-boxDomain(s)EukaryotaGOGO:0006396 GO:0005730SOSO:0000593PDB structuresPDBe In molecular biology, Small nucleolar RNA snR61/Z1/Z11 refers to a group of related non-coding RNA (ncRNA) molecules which function in the biogenesis of other small nuclear RNAs (snRNAs). These small nucleolar RNAs (snoRNAs) are modifying RNAs a...
Czech pornographic actress (born 1987) Tarra WhiteWhite in 2007BornMartina Mrakviová[1] (1987-11-19) 19 November 1987 (age 36)Ostrava, CzechoslovakiaOther namesSusie[2]Chrissy[2]OndrejaTarra Light[2]Height5 ft 7 in (1.70 m)[2]Children3 Tarra White (born Martina Mrakviová,[1] 19 November 1987[2]) is a Czech pornographic actress. Biography White was born in Ostrava, Czechoslovakia.[2] At the age of five, ...
Sebuah sinyal biner, juga dikenal sebagai sinyal logik, adalah sinyal digital dengan dua level yang dapat dibedakan Sinyal digital adalah sebuah jenis sinyal yang memiliki nilai berbentuk digital. Nilai bilangan pada sinyal digital hanya bilangan biner. Bentuknya juga hanya gelombang pulsa. Sinyal digital dapat mengirimkan informasi dengan kecepatan tinggi secara berulang tanpa mengurangi kualitas dan kuantitas informasi. Daya kerja sinyal digital rendah dan memiliki kinerja maksimal. Dalam s...
C. 1530–1535 painting by Titian Allegory of MarriageArtistTitianYearc. 1530–1535MediumOil on canvasDimensions123 cm × 107 cm (48 in × 42 in)LocationLouvre, ParisAccessionINV 754; MR 506 The Allegory of Marriage (French: Allégorie Conjugale), also titled the Allegory of Separation (Allégorie de la séparation), and formerly known as the Allegory of Alfonso d'Avalos (Allégorie d'Alphonse d'Avalos), is an oil painting by Titian, made about...
Альбом Get Rich or Die Tryin’ американского рэпера 50 Cent стал лучшим по продажам в 2003 году. Список лучших альбомов США 2003 года (Billboard Year End Charts) — итоговый список наиболее популярных альбомов журнала Billboard по данным продаж за 2003 год[1][2]. Содержание 1 История 2 См. также 3 Прим...
The Commission for the Fight against Genocide's headquarters in Kigali in 2012 The National Commission for the Fight against Genocide (CNLG) is a Rwandan organisation that is concerned with the 1994 Rwanda genocide. They are involved with studying what happened. Their mission is to preserve the memory of the crimes and to study how it can be avoided. The organisation is mandated by section 179 of the Constitution of Rwanda.[1] One of their actions was to create the Kigali Genocide Mem...
This article relies excessively on references to primary sources. Please improve this article by adding secondary or tertiary sources. Find sources: Feel the Blade – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (February 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this template message)2008 studio album by Legion of the DamnedFeel the BladeStudio album by Legion of the DamnedReleased4 January 2008 (2008-01-04)RecordedStage One Studio in G...
Cuban boxer Andy CruzAndy Cruz in May 2023Born (1995-08-12) August 12, 1995 (age 28)Matanzas, CubaNationalityCubanStatisticsWeight(s)LightweightHeight1.68 m (5 ft 6 in)StanceOrthodox Boxing record[1]Total fights1Wins1Wins by KO0Losses0Draws0No contests0 Medal record Men's amateur boxing Representing Cuba Event 1st 2nd 3rd Olympic Games 1 0 0 World Championships 3 0 0 Pan American Games 2 0 0 Total 6 0 0 Olympic Games 2020 Tokyo Lightweight World Amateur Champ...
English politician and noble This article includes a list of general references, but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations. Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations. (August 2014) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Arms of Clifton of Clifton, Nottinghamshire (Baron Clifton of Leighton Bromswold): Sable semée of cinquefoils and a lion rampant argent Gervase Clifton, 1st Baron Clifton (c. 1570 – 14 October 1618) was an English n...